Compositor B, The Pavier Quartos, and Copy
Spellings
by
William S. Kable
Printed books are the chief textual witnesses for the majority of
Renaissance English literature. Lacking authorial manuscripts, our
knowledge of the words from the author's pen comes to us through the
medium of print. Modern bibliographical researches have only begun to
explore fully the possibilities of reducing this distance between the author's
pen and the extant printed text by investigating the process by which the
author's words reached print. In this beginning, however, the printing house
compositor has already emerged as a significant factor in the transformation
of manuscript into printed text. Compositorial study concentrates on the
identification and evaluation of the work of these men, and a comprehensive
spelling analysis is one of the first necessities in any thoroughgoing
compositor study.
I.
In June 1920, Thomas Satchell announced the discovery of two
distinct spelling patterns which indicated the presence of two identifiable
compositors involved in the setting of type for Macbeth in the
First Folio Shakespeare. Satchell named these compositors A and B and
outlined their significant spelling patterns based on his researches in
Macbeth. Compositor A was recognized principally by the
spellings doe, goe, and here;
Compositor B, by do, go, heere.[1]
E. E. Willoughby extended Satchell's identification of A and B to
other parts of the Folio and suggested the hypothesis that "since in the
portion we have investigated there are many passages that are not
characteristic of either of them [A and B], it seems probable that there was
also another pair of compositors at work."[2]

Alice Walker attempted a start at assessing the quality of the work of
A and B with special reference to I Henry IV, but little real
progress was made at refining compositorial study in the Folio until
Charlton Hinman undertook his exhaustive study of the many copies of the
Folio at the Folger Shakespeare Library.[3] Hinman's use of type and case
identification added a new dimension to compositor study in the Folio. In
1957 he was able to announce the discovery and isolation of a fifth,
apprentice, Compositor E, whose work had clouded the picture in the
Tragedies.[4] Until Hinman was able
to demonstrate the presence of this fifth compositor on the basis of
indisputable physical evidence, the similarity of E's most important spelling
habits to those of B had caused investigators to assign E's work to B. The
coincidence that do, go, and
heere were
both B's and E's preferred spellings of these
words delayed the separation of their work until Hinman's new approach
was applied to the problem.
Publication of Hinman's completed study of the printing of the Folio
provides the first step toward a complete investigation of all five Jaggard
compositors involved in setting type for the Folio.[5] His findings in respect to the three
key
words for compositor identification can be summarized as follows:
- Compositor A — doe, goe, here
- Compositor B — do, go, heere
- Compositor C — doe, goe, heere
- Compositor D — doe, goe (with tolerance for
do
and go found in copy), here
- Compositor E — do, go, heere (with early tolerance for
copy
spellings)
With the exception of E, who joined Jaggard's staff only in the later stages
of work on the Folio, Compositors A, B, C and D make up a four-man
staff which could be expected to carry out the composition in Jaggard's
shop under normal conditions. Much work still remains to be done to
produce an exact basis for identifying C and D in the Folio, and qualitative
evaluation of all five compositors is needed. The above listing of
do,
go,
here habits does,
however, give
the basic means of identifying those workmen who can be hypothesized to
have made up Jaggard's compositorial staff during the setting of most of the
Folio,
and that listing can be used as a provisional limit to Jaggard's compositorial
staff.
[6]
The ten Shakespearean and pseudo-Shakespearean plays which make
up the group called the Pavier quartos were stumbling blocks to
bibliographical and textual researchers before the discovery and
demonstration in 1908-1910 that they are all simple page-for-page reprints
produced in Isaac Jaggard's shop for Thomas Pavier in 1619.[7] Several of these reprints had even
been
incorrectly identified as the true first editions of their texts on the basis of
substantive variants. The following table lists the Pavier titles and the copy
from which they were set. The printers' names are appended to the list of
copy to show the diversity of copy which served for the Paviers.[8]
Pavier Texts
|
Greg # |
Copy
|
Printed by:
|
2 Henry VI |
119(c) |
Q1(1594) |
Thomas Creede |
3 Henry VI |
138(c) |
O1(1595) |
Peter Short |
Pericles |
284(d) |
Q3(1611) |
Simon Stafford |
A Yorkshire Tragedy |
272(b) |
Q1(1608) |
Richard Bradock |
The Merchant of Venice |
172(b) |
Q1(1600) |
James Roberts |
Merry Wives of Windsor |
187(b) |
Q1(1602) |
Thomas Creede |
King Lear |
265(b) |
Q1(1608) |
Nicholas Okes |
Henry V |
165(c) |
Q1(1600) |
Thomas Creede |
Sir John Oldcastle |
166(b) |
Q1(1600) |
Valentine Simmes |
Midsummer Night's Dream |
170(b) |
Q1(1600) |
Richard Bradock(?) |
After the initial argumentative flurry over the identification of the true
nature of the Pavier quartos, Shakespearean bibliographical and textual
study has in general ignored them or, at best, hastily dismissed them as
simple reprints. It is true that there is little chance that the Paviers will
produce any startlingly new substantive witness to the Shakespeare text, but
since these ten reprints are all products of Jaggard's printing house just a
few years before the production of the all-important Folio in that very shop,
they offer an excellent opportunity
to see Jaggard's workmen dealing with dramatic texts. This opportunity is
made all the more significant by the fact that all ten plays are reprints from
earlier editions which are extant. The exact copy and the resulting Jaggard
text can be compared side by side.
D. F. McKenzie pointed the way to the significance of the Pavier
quartos in a preliminary study of "Compositor B's Role in The
Merchant of Venice Q2 (1619)."[9] Although his spelling tests were
based on
the word list of Alice Walker, who in turn compiled her list before
Compositor E had been distinguished from B, McKenzie's statistics capably
demonstrated that Jaggard's Compositor B as recognized in the Folio had
in fact set the entire Pavier quarto of The Merchant of
Venice.
It must be remembered that E, the only compositor who could easily be
confused with B, did not enter the shop until 1622. McKenzie was then able
to observe exactly what B did with his copy, Merchant Q1
(1600). His study was divided between the few substantive alterations which
B gratuitously introduced into the text and a brief discussion of B's
"alteration of accidentals."
McKenzie did not, however, suggest another important use which can
be made of the Pavier reprints and the controls which exist for the study of
these texts. In the past, compositorial identification has concentrated on the
peculiarities of the workman, on the individual mark which he leaves on the
copy he sets. Jaggard's Compositor B, for example, is known primarily for
his strong spelling habits, do, go, and
heere. Texts set by B are noted for the virtual invariability
of
these spellings. There is another aspect, however, to compositor study
which has been neglected in the concentration on the features of a
workman's habits which are his badge and serve to identify him from his
fellows. After the bibliographer has determined with assurance that a given
compositor did in fact set a given portion of text, the value of the
identifying spellings is exhausted. A do, go, or
heere in a Folio text may be of use in assigning that text to
Compositor B, but its value ends there.
In many instances, it would be useful to be able to penetrate the layer
of compositorial spellings and go behind the workman to the features of his
copy. The study of a new class of words in relation to individual
compositors may enable the textual investigator to do just that. These words
are those for which the compositor's spelling treatment is recognizable but
not so pronounced as to be called an invariable habit. For example, a
do in a B text can represent any form of the
word (
do,
doe,
doo) in his copy.
B's
strong habit virtually blocks out any trace of the copy spellings for this
word. If, however, a new group of words can be discovered for which the
compositor does not have an invariable habit, but rather a weak preference
or even indifference, these words can then be used, not as identifying marks
showing the presence of the compositor, but as a means of seeing through
that compositor to his copy. If the compositor has no set preferential
spelling for a given word but varies his spellings in some relation to the
variations in his copy, the forms of that word in his texts will directly
reflect the forms which were in the copy. If the compositor does have some
preferential spelling for a given word but is influenced by his copy to set
another form from time to time, these occasional occurrences of the
non-preferential form(s) will again reveal copy spellings. Once a group of
such words can be established for a given compositor,
certain projections of expected copy spellings can then be made using the
spellings in this new group. Such a projection could be of the greatest
possible utility in situations where the copy is unknown, lost, or
disputed.
The Pavier quartos provide just the controlled situation necessary to
make it possible to discover whether this hypothetical group of words does
in fact exist within a compositor's total spelling pattern and to establish a
broader picture of the nature of compositorial spelling habits, including
weak preferences and the cases of relative indifference. First, however, it
is necessary to identify the compositor(s) at work in the Paviers.
The following tables represent the results of a count of the
do, go, here spellings throughout
the
Paviers.
|
2H6 |
3H6 |
PER |
YT |
MV |
MW |
KL |
H5 |
SJO |
MND |
Total |
Do |
45 |
47 |
76 |
15 |
101 |
55 |
98 |
48 |
89 |
102 |
676 |
Doe |
2 |
0 |
11 |
1 |
12 |
1 |
4 |
0 |
1 |
4 |
36 |
|
2H6 |
3H6 |
PER |
YT |
MV |
MW |
KL |
H5 |
SJO |
MND |
Total |
Go |
56 |
16 |
10 |
3 |
41 |
29 |
37 |
20 |
38 |
21 |
271 |
Goe |
5 |
3 |
11 |
5 |
16 |
23 |
21 |
1 |
3 |
17 |
105 |
|
2H6 |
3H6 |
PER |
YT |
MV |
MW |
KL |
H5 |
SJO |
MND |
Total |
Heere |
57 |
38 |
48 |
11 |
62 |
29 |
38 |
16 |
58 |
32 |
389 |
Here |
22 |
11 |
21 |
3 |
15 |
24 |
33 |
8 |
25 |
29 |
191 |
Although the strength of do and the more or less strong
preferences for go and heere seem to point to
Compositor B, the real value of these tables lies in demonstrating the
dangerous invalidity of overly simplified
statistical compilations of spelling evidence. Two important factors have
been completely omitted from this set of tables, factors without which no
really significant spelling analysis can be made of the Pavier quartos.
First, there is no recognition of the copy spellings which lie behind
these statistics. For example, the evidence for do (101
occurrences) as opposed to doe (twelve occurrences) in
The Merchant of Venice seems at first to conflict with
Compositor B's almost invariable do habit. A closer look at
this
group of spellings with the additional evidence of the spellings of the copy
text, however, makes an extremely strong case in favor of Compositor B's
having set The Merchant. Of the twelve doe
spellings in the Pavier, all twelve reproduce B's non-habitual
doe as found in copy, Q1 (1600). Of the 101
do
spellings in the Pavier, only five were found in copy and ninety-six
represent gratuitous changes of doe to do on the
part
of the compositor. To state these facts yet another way, the compositor of
Pavier Merchant found the word do spelled
doe 108 times and do five times in his copy.
He
reproduced all five do spellings and changed ninety-six of the
108 doe to do leaving only twelve
doe
forms found in his copy. This additional evidence of the copy spellings
overwhelmingly shows the strength of the do spelling in the
habits of the compositor who set the Pavier Merchant even
though the raw statistics point to a partial use of the doe
spelling.
This distinction between the raw numerical totals in the first case and
the weighted strength provided by the evidence of copy spellings is
essentially similar to that between quantitative and qualitative
evidence.[10] The quantities of
spellings, do (101) and doe (twelve), did in fact
point in the general direction of B's habit, but the quality of the evidence
only becomes apparent after a review of the copy spellings reveals that
ninety-six changes of doe to do are hidden in
the
raw statistics. Given the relative value of any one change over any one
instance of following copy, the quality of the evidence makes the case for
B one of certainty. Failure to consider the copy spellings greatly weakens
and obscures the genuine evidence. From this example, an important
principle governing the study of compositorial habits can be formulated. In
compositorial analysis of texts for which the copy is known and available,
all
consideration of variant spellings must include an examination of the copy
spellings.
The second factor which was omitted from the first group of tables
is that of the influence of justification on a compositor's spellings. It
has long been recognized that, as McKerrow said, early compositors "had
. . . a means of justifying the lines of type which is denied to modern
compositors, namely, by varying the spelling of words."
[11] If this is true, and one of the
overall
results of the examination of the Pavier spellings is to demonstrate
graphically the truth of McKerrow's axiom, evidence of spellings in
justified lines must be carefully separated from the evidence in short
lines.
[12]
In any given line, the possible influence of justification on the
spellings is a matter for debate. The possibility of such influence must,
however, cause one to segregate the spelling evidence which could be
contaminated by the need for justification. Take, for example, the statistics
for go/goe in King Lear. The simple counts
record
thirty-seven go and twenty-one goe spellings.
When
the possible effects of justification are taken into consideration, these
statistics become go thirty-one times plus six justified
occurrences and goe seven times plus fourteen justified
occurrences. The original ratio of thirty-seven go to
twenty-one
goe spellings is rather questionable evidence for B's
go as a preferential spelling. When the justified and,
therefore,
possibly deceptive spellings are taken away, the ratio of thirty-one
go to seven goe becomes much more
convincing.
When this allowance for justification is
combined with a consideration of the copy spellings, the total strength of
the evidence emerges. Go was found ten times in
Lear Q1 (1608), seven times in short lines and three times in
justified lines. All ten of these go spellings are retained in the
Pavier Lear. On the other hand, Lear Q1 contained thirty-one
goe spellings in short lines and seventeen in justified lines.
Of
the thirty-one long spellings, twenty-four were changed to go.
Apparently, therefore, the factor of justification caused the compositor's
preference for the short go spelling to be obscured in the
justified occurrences of this word. A second principle to govern spelling
analysis is that the possible influence of justification on variant spellings
must be acknowledged, and spellings in long lines must be separated from
other spellings in any statistical compilation.
The following set of tables represents a restatement of the evidence
for do, go, and here in the light of these two
principles. Note that the words are recorded only in relation to copy
spellings and that evidence in justified lines is separated from the general
statistics. In these tables, J stands for justified, or at least
long
line, occurrences.
Copy→ |
1619 |
2H6 |
3H6 |
PER |
YT |
MV |
MW |
KL |
H5 |
SJO |
MND |
Totals |
Do→Do |
41 |
12 |
15 |
3 |
3 |
45 |
22 |
48 |
57 |
24 |
270 |
|
J4 |
|
J6 |
J4 |
J2 |
J10 |
J5 |
|
J15 |
J5 |
J51 |
Do→ |
2 |
|
|
|
|
|
|
|
1 |
|
3 |
Doe |
|
|
J2 |
J1 |
|
J1 |
J1 |
|
|
|
J5 |
Doe→ |
|
35 |
34 |
6 |
87 |
|
59 |
|
13 |
69 |
303 |
Do |
|
|
J21 |
J2 |
J9 |
|
J12 |
|
J4 |
J4 |
J52 |
Doe→ |
|
|
2 |
|
8 |
|
1 |
|
|
1 |
12 |
Doe |
|
|
J7 |
|
J4 |
|
J2 |
|
|
J3 |
J16 |
Copy→ |
1619 |
2H6 |
3H6 |
PER |
YT |
MV |
MW |
KL |
H5 |
SJO |
MND |
Totals |
Go→ |
49 |
13 |
|
3 |
|
15 |
7 |
18 |
20 |
4 |
129 |
Go |
J3 |
|
J1 |
|
|
J4 |
J3 |
|
J10 |
|
J21 |
Go→ |
2 |
1 |
|
|
1 |
1 |
|
|
|
|
5 |
Goe |
|
|
J1 |
|
|
J3 |
|
|
J2 |
|
J6 |
Goe→ |
4 |
3 |
5 |
|
34 |
9 |
24 |
2 |
5 |
16 |
102 |
Go |
|
|
J4 |
|
J17 |
J1 |
J3 |
|
J3 |
J1 |
J19 |
Goe→ |
3 |
2 |
4 |
5 |
13 |
16 |
7 |
1 |
|
17 |
68 |
Goe |
|
|
J6 |
|
J2 |
J3 |
J14 |
|
J1 |
|
J26 |
Copy→ |
1619 |
2H6 |
3H6 |
PER |
YT |
MV |
MW |
KL |
H5 |
SJO |
MND |
Totals |
Heere→ |
2 |
19 |
23 |
7 |
51 |
5 |
7 |
1 |
8 |
11 |
134 |
Heere |
|
|
J7 |
J2 |
J5 |
J2 |
|
|
J2 |
|
J18 |
Heere→ |
|
3 |
2 |
|
6 |
|
1 |
|
|
1 |
13 |
Here |
|
J1 |
J1 |
J1 |
J3 |
J2 |
|
|
J1 |
|
J9 |
Here→ |
51 |
19 |
14 |
1 |
6 |
19 |
25 |
15 |
42 |
19 |
211 |
Heere |
J4 |
|
J4 |
J1 |
|
J3 |
J6 |
|
J6 |
J2 |
J26 |
Here→ |
19 |
7 |
16 |
2 |
6 |
21 |
18 |
8 |
17 |
23 |
137 |
Here |
J3 |
|
J2 |
|
|
J1 |
J14 |
|
J7 |
J5 |
J32 |
Only in these complete tables which take the copy spellings and
justification into consideration does the overwhelming evidence for
Compositor B as the man who set type for the entire set of the Paviers
become apparent. The already small total of thirty-six spellings in the first,
overly simplified, tables becomes even more insignificant when it
can be observed that of these thirty-six occurrences of
doe,
twenty-eight (and of that twenty-eight, sixteen justified) reproduce copy
spellings. In all ten plays, there are only three non-copy, non-justified
doe spellings as opposed to 303 such
do
forms.
The go/goe evidence was not at all clear-cut in the first
table but here is shown to be significantly in favor of Compositor B's
go. Of the 105 goe spellings in the Paviers,
ninety-four (and of that ninety-four, twenty-six justified) reproduce copy
spellings. There are only five non-justified, non-copy goe
spellings while there are 101 similar go forms.
The here/heere ratio was also not clear-cut in the
simplified table. With the additional factors considered, however, 169 of the
191 here spellings turn out to derive from the copy used for
the
Paviers. Of that 169, thirty-two were in justified lines. The thirteen
non-copy, non-justified here spellings must be compared with
211 similar heere forms.
The total number of B spellings gratuitously introduced into the
Paviers is 615. In view of the token number of twenty-one changes in the
opposite direction, the presence of B's hand throughout the Paviers is
evident. It is also important to note that the few aberrant non-B changes are
not significantly grouped, and in no instance do they suggest the presence
of another compositor.
The examination of the do, go,
here spellings in the Pavier quartos shows the special
significance of change, that is, the altering of copy in contrast to the
following of copy. The spelling pattern of go/goe in A
Midsummer Night's Dream is an excellent example of the relative
importance of spelling change. The raw evidence in the Paviers is almost
evenly divided between twenty go and seventeen
goe. The direction of the changes from copy which produced
this result tells a far different story. Of the four go spellings
in
copy, there is not one instance of change to goe. Of the
thirty-three goe spellings in copy, however, almost half,
sixteen,
were changed to go. On the surface, the sixteen changes in
thirty-three occurrences of goe might appear to express a
certain
degree of compositorial indifference to the spelling of this word. When
viewed as a part of the overall pattern of B's spellings and in
relation to the four go forms, all of which remain unchanged,
this change of sixteen out of thirty-three goe to
go
is far from expressing indifference. Rather, it indicates as strongly as
possible the compositor's preference for the go form. A
single
change is of much more significance in assessing a compositor's preferences
than is a single case of his following copy. The sixteen changes of
goe to go tell a great deal more about the
compositor's preference than do the seventeen instances of following
copy.
This examination of do, go,
here
spellings in the Paviers has accomplished two things. First, it has shown the
care which must be employed in avoiding over-simplification in the
expression of statistical spelling evidence. Spelling analysis must be done
in conjunction with an examination, where possible, of copy spellings, and
the possible influence of justification on variant spellings must be
considered at all times. Secondly, the introduction of these refinements into
the analysis of do, go, here
spellings has
brought convincing strength to the demonstration of the hypothesis that
Jaggard's Compositor B did in fact set the whole of the text of the Pavier
quartos.
II.
Armed with this warning against over-simplification and, at the same
time, strengthened by the assurance that B's hand is in evidence throughout
the Paviers, I collated all of the Pavier texts against their copy and
compiled word counts for over 500 individual words. This mass of
statistical evidence was then analyzed in terms of orthographic groups
which display significant spelling features. Although I do not wish to
reproduce these tedious reports of observed statistics, I propose to give here
a list of the groups of words I examined and one sample report in order to
make the nature of my study known to those who may be interested in
consulting it.[13]
The groups of spellings considered were as follows:
- 1. Internal -A-/-AU-
- 2. Initial DE-/DI-
- 3. Internal Consonants: -D-/-DD-, -R-/-RR-, -T-/-TT-
- 4. Final -D/-DE
- 5. Initial E-/I- before -N-/-M-
- 6. Internal -EA-/-EE-
- 7. Internal -EA-/-EI-
- 8. Internal -EE-/-IE-
- 9. Final -E/-EE
- 10. Final -F/-FE
- 11. Final -G
- 12. Internal -I-/-Y-
- 13. Final -IE/-Y
- 14. Final -K/-CK/-QUE
- 15. Final -L
- 16. Final -M
- 17. Final -N
- 18. Internal -O-/-OO-
- 19. Final -P
- 20. Final -R
- 21. Final -S/-SSE
- 22. Final -T
- 23. Final -W
- 24. Final -X
- 25. Reflexive Pronouns
One of the largest of these groups of words is that involving
-ie/-y endings. Before one can attempt to generalize about the
eighty-eight
words in this group for which there is sufficient evidence to permit valid
statistical conclusions, they must be examined in sub-groups. First, those
words ending in a vowel plus
-ie or
-y will be
considered. After that, the words ending in a consonant plus
-ie/-y will be examined in two groups defined by penultimate
consonants.
All of the following words display B's extremely strong preference
for final -y in conjunction with a preceding vowel:
away |
enjoy |
pay |
betray |
joy |
pray |
bewray |
lay |
say |
boy |
may |
stay |
day |
nay |
way |
Without exception, B changes
-ie and
-ye forms
of
these words to
-y and retains all of the
-y forms
found in copy.
The following words ending in -ie/-y after penultimate
letters b, c, d, g,
k, l, m, n,
p,
r, t, and v regularly display B's
preference for -y endings:
b: rugby |
willingly |
t: authority |
c: fancy |
m: enemy |
beauty |
mercy |
n: any |
charity |
d: already |
company |
city |
bloody |
deny |
cruelty |
body |
honey |
duty |
lady |
many |
fifty |
needy |
money |
forty |
ready |
villany |
guilty |
speedy |
p: happy |
honesty |
g: clergy |
r: beggery |
liberty |
k: lucky |
carry |
loyalty |
l: daily |
contrary |
majesty |
dearly |
country |
mighty |
early |
cry |
nativity |
fly |
dowry |
necessity |
heartily |
every |
petty |
heavenly |
fury |
pity |
holy |
glory |
pretty |
kingly |
history |
quality |
only |
marry |
safety |
presently |
merry |
thirty |
princely |
misery |
twenty |
quickly |
sorry |
university |
reply |
tarry |
v: envy |
suddenly |
very |
heavy |
truly |
victory |
|
Preferring final
-y for all of the words on this list, B
regularly
changes the bulk of the
-ie forms to
-y and
almost
invariably reproduces the
-y forms found in copy. A small
and
relatively regular percentage of
-ie spellings slip through from
copy into B's texts, but these carry-overs always occur in conjunction with
B's definite preference for
-y.
There are two consonants which do not fall into the above group and,
in fact, display a distinctly contrasting pattern. These consonants are
f and s. Defie stands in B's copy
six
times and all of these six occurrences are reproduced by B. B's copy spells
satisfy with final -ie all five times it occurs. B
follows copy for all of these forms. Signifie is so spelled in
six
(+J1) occurrences in copy. All of these forms are reproduced by B. All
four (+J1) occurrences of busy are spelled
busie
both in copy and in B's texts. B reproduces the seven (+J1)
curtesie spellings in copy, changes three (+J1)
curtesie forms to courtesie, and reproduces
three
courtesie spellings. All seven easie spellings in
copy
are retained in B's texts. Heresie is reproduced from copy the
three times the word occurs. One heresie form is set in place
of copy's herisie. The seven occurrences of
jealousy are spelled with final -ie both in copy
and
in B's texts. Lowsie is reproduced from copy three times and
once changed to lowsy.
All of the words recorded for f and s plus
-ie/-y display an uncanny pattern of strongly favoring final
-ie in both copy and B's texts. It is likely that the anomalous
-fie/-sie endings result from the use of the fi
and
long-s/i ligatures. Since all of the Pavier copy produced by
various printers agrees with B's use of -fie/-sie in spite of the
general -ie/-y treatment, the fi and
si
ligatures were apparently used with some consistency in order to make the
supply of plain f's and s's in the case last as
long
as possible. The unanimity of the printers in following this procedure makes
it appear to be a general convention for stretching the supply of type.
B displays, therefore, a special tolerance for final -ie
after
-f- and -s-. It is important to stress the word
tolerance because this special feature of B's treatment of final
-ie/-y cannot be termed a preference. Without being able to
observe B's reaction to -fy and -sy in copy, we
cannot equate his use of -fie/-sie with his demonstrable
general
preference for final -y as evidenced by the majority of the
evidence given above. Indeed, the only change in that last group of
-f- and -s- is that of one of the four
lowsie forms to lowsy. Taken by itself, this
shred
of evidence points to the fact that B's preferential spelling was final
-y but that his tolerance for ligatures with -ie
was
surprisingly strong. It must also be pointed out that B's tolerance for
-fie and -sie is not simply a tendency to give
in to
copy spellings when they are weighted against his
preferences
as were the
-fie and
-sie copy spellings against
his
general preference for
-y. In the group which displays B's
preference for final
-y, many individual words had large
proportions of
-ie endings in copy but were consistently set
by
B with final
-y.
Happy was found in copy
seventeen
(+J1) to five in favor of
-ie but becomes twenty-two (+J1)
happy in B's texts.
Victory,
guilty,
nativity, and
thirty were all uniformly spelled
with
final
-ie in copy but with final
-y in B's
texts.
To summarize B's spelling patterns for words ending in
-ie/-y, B consistently prefers final -y after
preceding
vowels and consonants with the exception of f and
s.
Although his preference for final -y involves both the
retention
of almost all -y endings and the introduction of
-y
endings for the vast majority of -ie forms in his copy, a
limited
percentage of -ie forms is carried over from copy into B's
texts.
These few -ie forms serve as an indication of copy spellings.
For the only exceptions to this general rule, -fie and
-sie, B displays an almost absolute tolerance for the
-ie endings which are consistently found in copy.
An examination of compositor B's group spellings has revealed both
the dangers of and benefits to be derived from the use of analogy in the
analysis of compositorial spellings. Although compositor identification in
the Folio has generally been limited to the use of variant spellings of
individual words, other compositor studies have employed classes of words
such as that formed by all words with -ie/-y endings. Since
investigators have employed these word classes in compositor identification,
it is of interest to examine the nature and extent of analogy in the overall
spelling pattern of a compositor.
There are several large groups of words which B treats in a unified
manner. His consistent use of final -e after penultimate
-f- is of little significance because the vast majority of copy
spellings are also -fe. B's consistent preference for
-esse forms of words varying between -es and
-esse is an individual habit which extends to all of the similar
words recorded in the Paviers. From the evidence of the Pavier spellings,
a reasonably safe projection can be made from the uniformity of B's
preference for -esse. It is, however, not demonstrable that the
very next word examined will not for some special reason depart from the
pattern and display the opposite tendency. The discovery of B's clearly
defined preference for deere might lead one to expect that B
would also prefer the yeere spelling. In fact, however, B's
preference for yeare is equally strong as his use of
deere. Word classes cannot, therefore,
be formed by the use of simple analogy.
Although word classes cannot be automatically formed of analogical
words, Compositor B's spellings are far from being patternless. Consistent
patterns can in fact be observed for groups of similar words.
The key to the problem is that the word groups can be formed only after an
examination of the individual words. For example, the real pattern of B's
treatment of final
-ie/-y becomes clear only after considering
the
individual words. Since the raw statistics indicate that B obviously prefers
final
-y for all but a handful of words, one's first reaction is
to
consider B's pattern a general preference for final-
y with the
tendency to accept a few aberrant
-ie endings found in copy.
These words might, therefore, be designated exceptions to the general rule
and nothing more. When the individual words are analyzed, however, the
so-called "exceptions" to the one rule are seen to behave according to a
strict rule of their own. All of the words which display B's tolerance for
final
-ie have penultimate
-f- or
-s- and
are probably governed by the
fi and
si
ligatures. The
exceptions to the hypothetical blanket rule are in fact
governed by a separate, clearly defined, rule of their own. Only by
reasoning from an examination of the individual words is it possible to
define accurately the limits of B's pattern.
There are many similar situations which show the importance of
building up word classes of a more limited sort. Internal
-ai-/-ay- spellings appear to be mixed until it is observed that
B's spellings split into an -ayle group and a more general
-ai- group. In this case, the subgroup displaying
-ay-
is formed by words having analogous endings. The other group is not
governed by any common feature other than the internal -ai-.
Internal -r-/-rr- variation, however, falls into two groups,
both
of which are composed of analogous words.
In summary, therefore, Compositor B's spelling patterns indicate that
analogy often governed his treatment of various spellings. At the same time,
however, untested analogy cannot be employed to define linguistic units for
compositor study. The exact nature and extent of these analogous groups
must be determined only as a result of the study of separate words.
Although analogy must be employed with extreme caution, one
important factor, aberration, should not be permitted to interfere with the
careful formation of word groups and classes. The Pavier statistics confirm
a widely held hypothesis that for some unknown reason a given compositor
will on rare occasions contradict even his strongest habit. The point here is
that these aberrations should not be allowed to obscure the valid evidence
of spelling patterns.
In the examination of do, go, and
here, for example, B was seen to go against his strong
do habit and set doe three times in the course
of the
Paviers in instances when he was affected neither by the need for
justification nor by a copy doe spelling. These three
aberrations
in
the course of ten plays and over 600 occurrences of the word must remain
unexplained and be written off as human variability.
To take another example, a word first analyzed in the Paviers, the
following statistics represent the word count for only. The
first
column represents copy spellings and the second the spelling of those words
in the Paviers.
- ONLIE — ONELY 1
- ONLY — ONELY 12
- ONELY — ONELY 33+J2
- ONELY — ONLY 1
B's unique aberration in setting
only for
onely
does
not have any force against the formulation of the general rule that B's
preferential spelling is clearly
onely.
Although the existence of these aberrations should not be allowed to
obscure clearly observable general patterns, the fact that single aberrant
spellings occur calls for caution in placing weight on any one individual
spelling. To paraphrase Fredson Bowers commenting on aberration in the
printing process, the phenomenon of an individual spelling is narrow
enough "to introduce the possibility that a recognized . . . [spelling
preference] is not being revealed but instead that we are examining only an
aberration — some human variation or failure that must be regarded
as
producing a sport."[14]
III.
The examination of the Pavier spellings has also produced valuable
statistics on many individual words of interest. Spellings which have been
proposed by previous investigators as characteristic B spellings are here
reviewed in the light of the information provided by the statistics covering
B's work in the Paviers. All of the words that have been proposed by
Satchell, Willoughby, Cauthen,[15] and
Walker[16] as aids in identifying B's
work have been observed in the Paviers. None of these investigators
considered B's habit spellings in direct relation to copy, and all four of
them were working before Hinman's isolation of Compositor E. When
reference is make to these spelling analyses,
therefore, it must be remembered that E spellings can have contaminated
statistics for B. Hinman has commented briefly on the relation of spelling
habits to copy.
[17] Several other words
are included in this section although they are not identifying features for the
recognition of B's presence in a given text. These words have been chosen
for their special value in indicating other features of B's total spelling
pattern and new ways in which spellings can reveal evidence about the
compositor's treatment of his copy.
1. been
As a part of his attempt to demonstrate that all of Folio
Lear was set by B, Cauthen presented the following table as
evidence that B's preferential spelling was bin.
A Spellings
|
B Spellings
|
Lear Spellings
|
beene (13) |
beene (4) |
beene (8) |
bene (3) |
bene (6) |
bene (1) |
bin (3) |
bin (24) |
bin (19) |
When viewed in relation to copy, however, B's treatment of
been cannot be defined in terms of a single preferential form.
These statistics show that B had no single preferential spelling of
been. In fact, he actively employed, that is followed from
copy
and introduced against copy, three forms:
bin,
bene,
and
beene. Although B does not have a single positive
preference, he consistently rejects the
been and
byn
spellings which stand in copy. In the case of each of his acceptable
spellings, B adopts about half the number of any given form found in copy.
Although B's tolerance of
bin forms found in copy is as high
as
that toward
bene and
beene, he introduces
bin least frequently in replacing other forms. Between
bene and
beene, B apparently prefers
bene, but the evidence is not strong. All in all,
been
is of little use in penetrating B's texts to his copy. The word serves,
however, to show the possibility of a compositor's having several operative
forms in his treatment of a given word, that is, various forms which he will
both accept, reject, and introduce in place of others found in copy.
2. blood (bloody)
Compositor B displays a marked preference for -oo-
forms of blood and bloody. Of the 103 (+J4)
-ou- forms in copy, B changes sixty-three (+J4) to
-oo-. At the same time, he retains sixty-one (+J5) of the
sixty-six (+J5) -oo- spellings found in copy. It is interesting
to
note that in spite of his clear-cut preference for -oo-,
Compositor B does allow four -ou- spellings in ten to come
through into his texts. In all ten plays, there are only five abberant changes
of -oo- to -ou- or about one in thirteen
occurrences
of the word.
3. brief
Cauthen's statistics suggest that B has a two to one preference for
briefe over breefe. In the Paviers, only one
justified
-ee- form occurs in copy and this is followed by B. Of the
twenty-one (+J3) -ie- forms in copy, B changes five to
-ee- and retains sixteen (+J3) as -ie-. From this
evidence, it would appear that any preference which these statistics point
toward is for -ee- over -ie-. To complete the
picture,
it would be necessary to be able to observe B's treatment of several
-ee- forms when found in copy.
4. choose
Alice Walker has suggested that B displays a preference for
choose in contrast to A's chuse. In the limited
evidence provided by the Paviers, B changes three of the five
chuse forms in copy to choose. Of the three
choose forms in copy, B retains two but sets one as
chuse. Walker's suggestion that B favored
choose
is confirmed by the evidence in the Paviers.
5. cousin
Taking the -o-/-ou- variation as the operative feature,
the
evidence provided by the Paviers fails to confirm Willoughby's and
Walker's suggestion that B preferred -o- forms over
-ou- spellings. Of the nine (+J1) -ou- spellings
in
copy, B retains eight and changes only one (+J1). Of the eleven (+J3)
-o- spellings in copy, B retains ten (+J3) and changes only
one.
6. dear
The variation of -ea-/-ee- is the operative feature in
dear. Of the sixty (+J12) -ea- forms found in
copy,
B changes twenty-three (+J6) to -ee-. He retains all
thirty-seven
(+J7) -ee- forms in copy. These statistics point clearly to his
preference for deere.
7. devil
Compositor B expresses a strong preference for the di-
form of devil. Of the eighteen (+J7) de- forms
in
copy, B changes seventeen (+J6) to di-. Only one (+J1)
non-preferential de- spelling slips through from copy into B's
text. All twenty-seven (+J9) di- forms in copy are retained
by
B.
8. forfeit
Forfeit provides another example, like
been,
of a situation where multiple alternative spellings
[forfait(e), forfet, and
forfeit(e)] were available to B. In this instance,
he
displays a double preference for forfet and
forfeit(e) and completely rejects the third
alternative.
At the same time, he fails to display any single clear-cut preference for
either of the two forms which he does use.
9. grant
Walker's suggestion that B preferred the grant form is
strongly confirmed by the Pavier spellings. He changes every one of the
eight (+J1) copy graunt spellings to grant and
retains all twelve (+J2) copy grant forms.
10. grief
Walker included greefe in her list of B's preferred
spellings. Of the thirty-eight (+J5) griefe spellings in copy,
B
changes seventeen (+J1) to greefe. Both greefe
forms in copy are retained. The greefe preference is
confirmed.
11. heart
B displays a marked preference for the -ea- form of
heart. Of the ninety-two (+J13) hart forms in
copy,
B changes seventy-three (+J6) to -ea-. He retains 111 (+J21)
of the 112 (+J24) -ea- forms found in copy. There is only
one
(+J3) anomalous change of -ea- to -a-. B's
-ea- preference is clearly indicated.
12. heaven
Alice Walker observed that B preferred uncapitalized
heaven up until Macbeth in the Folio. In the
Paviers, B changes eight of the nine capitalized Heaven forms
to lower case and reproduces thirty-six (+J2) uncapitalized forms found in
copy.
13. honey
Alice Walker first observed B's preference for the hony
spelling of this word. He changes all three honey spellings in
copy to hony and reproduces the four short forms which stand
in his copy. There is one anomalous change of hony to
honny.
14. hour
Cauthen and Walker both indicate that B's preferred spelling was
houre. Of the thirteen (+J2) -ow- spellings of
hour in copy, B changes eleven (+J2) to -ou-.
One
hower and one houre-glasse spellings are
reproduced
by B from his copy. He retains all thirty-four (+J4) -ou-
forms
found in copy. The Pavier evidence indicates that B's preference for
-ou- was quite strong.
15. "lushious"
The one occurrence of lushious in both copy and Pavier
texts is included in this discussion because Alice Walker put great weight
upon the coincidence of the lushious spelling in Q1 and Folio
Othello. She used this coincidence as evidence that Folio
Othello was derived from a marked copy of Q1 (1622). "It
is
ridiculous to suppose that anomalies like 'timerous' (I.i.76), 'lushious'
(I.iii.346) and 'pudled' (III.iv.144) passed from a fair copy, via a prompt
book, to a manuscript prepared by a book-keeper with (at best) only half an
eye on the prompt-book, and so into Okes's quarto, thereby representing a
common legacy in the quarto and Folio from some common
ancestor."[18] If Miss Walker had
checked the Concordance, she would have found two recorded uses of
luscious, in the Othello passage in question
(I.iii.354
— — Globe numbering) and in A Midsummer Night's
Dream (II.i.251). Both Midsummer Q1
(1600) and the Pavier Q2 (1619) contain the lushious spelling
(C2v in both Qq). Far from being anomalous, the
lushious
form is common to the compositors of Q1 Midsummer, Q1
Othello, and Jaggard's B. It is interesting to note that the
Midsummer lushious is changed to luscious on
N3v in the Folio. Hinman assigns this page to Compositor
C.[19] The point of this example is that
any
common spelling, however "anomalous" it may appear, must be used with
extreme caution in attempting to demonstrate direct derivation of one text
from another.
16. madam
Alice Walker suggested that madam is a characteristic
B
spelling whereas madame is characteristic of Compositor A.
Disregarding the
variation of
-d-/-dd- and concentrating on
-m/-me,
the evidence of the Paviers is somewhat mixed. B's copy is slanted in favor
of
-m (fifty-two +J7) over
-me (twenty-seven
+J5).
Of the twenty-seven (+J5)
-me forms, B changes eleven
(+J2)
to
-m and retains sixteen (+J3). On the other hand, a small
proportion, seven (+J1) of fifty-two (+J7),
-m copy spellings
are changed in the opposite direction to
-me. B's general
preference is indeed for
-m, but there are a number of
aberrant
changes of
-m to
-me.
17. mistress
Five variant spellings of mistress are found in the copy
used for the Paviers: mistres (eight +J1),
mistris
(twenty-two +J10), mistresse (forty-seven +J11),
mistrisse (one +J1), and misteris (thirty-five
+J5).
Of these five spellings, B prefers mistris and
mistresse and rejects the other three. Although Alice Walker
assigned mistresse to A and mistris to B as
characteristic spellings, B's treatment of the word in the Paviers indicates
that he employed both forms. On the surface, simple numerical superiority
does favor the -is form, but this is a result of the numerical
superiority of -is forms in copy.
18. power
Cauthen observed that B's texts contained both power
(18)
and powre (7).[20] In the
Paviers, however, B changes the two powre forms in copy
to
power and retains all twenty-six of the power
spellings which stand in his copy, thus indicating a preference for
power.
19. prithee
Alice Walker noted B's constant use of the spelling
prethee in the early sections of the Folio. In the Paviers, B
normalized a total of twenty-six (+J6) occurrences of ten different spellings
of prithee all to his preferential prethee. He
follows
one copy prithee, sets one justified copy prithe
as
prithee, and follows one copy prethe. B's habit
in
the Paviers coincides, therefore, with Walker's observation of his treatment
of prithee in the early parts of the Folio.
20. proud
The Paviers confirm Walker's assignment of proud as
B's
preferential form. B changes the one copy prowd and one
(+J1)
proude to proud and retains twelve occurrences
of
the latter form from copy.
21. scarce
The evidence supplied by the Paviers confirms the observation that
B had a strong preference for the -se form of
scarce.
He changes all eighteen (+J2) -ce forms in copy to
-se and retains all five -se forms which stand
in
copy.
22. traitor
Willoughby and Walker both stress B's preference for
traitor over traytor, and the Paviers confirm
this
preference. Of the twenty-five (+J4) traytor forms in copy,
B
changes twenty-five (+J1) to traitor. All thirty-five (+J3)
-i- forms in copy are retained by B. The only one (+J1)
traytor form in B's texts is derived from copy.
23. vile
Cauthen assigned the vile spelling to Compositor A and
vilde to B. B changes all five vild spellings in
copy
to vilde and one copy vilde to
vile. He
reproduces the two (+J1) other vilde spellings found in
copy.
24. wee'l (etc.)
Alice Walker suggested that B had a distinctive preference for
wee'l (etc.) spellings as opposed to wee'le
(etc.).
The following discussion breaks these forms down by person.
The various compositors who set the texts used for Pavier copy and
Compositor B display a surprising unanimity on the spelling of
I'll. There are 124 (+J13) instances of ile in
both
copy and Pavier texts. One aberrant yle form found in copy
is
changed to ile. B's one deviation from the standard form
occurs
in a justified line in which he sets I'le for
Ile.
He'll, she'll, we'll, and
you'll occur in sufficient numbers to demonstrate B's strong
preferences for final 'l forms of these words. Several variant
factors are involved in these words. The use of the apostrophe, the ending
(-l or -le) and the doubling (in the case of
he'll, she'll and we'll) of the first
e are all separate alternatives operative for these words. B's
uniform preferences are for the doubling of the initial e, the
use
of the apostrophe, and the omission of the final -e.
Of the eighteen (+J2) times B encounters heele in
copy,
he changes eighteen (+J1) of these forms to his preferential
hee'l. In the one justified instance, B changes
heele
to heel. In the two instances in which he encountered
hee'le both are changed to hee'l. The one
contraction introduced by B changes he will to
hee'l. One aberrant hele in B's copy comes
through
as an unusual heele in his text.
Of the five sheele forms in copy, B changes four to
shee'l and sets one as shee'll. He changes all
four
copy shee'le spellings to his preferential
shee'l. The one copy
shee'l is reproduced by
B. Of
the two
sheel forms in copy, B changes one to
shee'l and carries the other over to his text.
We'll is a more complex word. B displays a preference
for wee'l but is subject to the influence of copy and more
prone
to deviate from his strict preference. B changes twenty-nine (+J3)
weele forms to wee'l. At the same time, he
changes
two justified weele forms to wee'll, fourteen
(+J2)
weele forms to weel, three weele
forms
to weell, one justified weele form to
weel and retains eight (+J3) weele forms found
in
copy. If the three wele spellings in copy, B changes one to
his
apparent preference wee'l but the other two to
weele. The one we'le form is changed to his
preferred wee'l as are the eight (+J1) occurrences of
wee'le. The two wee'l forms are reproduced
from
copy. Of the two (+J1) weel spellings in copy, one (+J1) is
changed to B's preferential wee'l and one is retained. Both
the
one weell and the one we wil
which are contracted by B become weel.
Compositor B displays a strong preference for you'l by
changing twenty (+J3) youle spellings to this form. Only
twice
does he reproduce youle from copy and in one unusual
instance
sets youle in place of his preferred you'l. One
other
you'l in copy is retained, and the two you'le
forms
are shortened to you'l.
With the exception of the Ile which is uniformly spelled
thusly, Compositor B displays a consistent spelling preference in favor of
hee'l, wee'l, and you'l. The only
variation comes in his tendency to depart occasionally from his strict habit
with respect to wee'l.
One additional word can be considered with this group. B's treatment
of contracted he is is as follows. Twice he sets
hee's
for copy he is. Of the eleven (+J4) hees in
copy,
B sets six (+J4) as hee's and five as he's. The
one
he's in copy is changed to hee's. Of the
seventeen
(+J3) hee's spellings in copy, B follows copy seventeen
(+J2)
times and changes one hee's to hee is in a
justified
line where these words stand at the very end of the line. B's preference is
clearly for the hee's spelling.
25. widow
The Pavier evidence confirms Alice Walker's suggestion that B's
preference is for widdow. Of the seven (+J1)
-d-
forms in copy, B changes three to -dd-. He retains eight
(+J2)
of the ten (+J2) -dd- forms found in copy. In addition, B
drops
the final -e found on one form in copy.
26. year
B's preference is clearly for the yeare spelling. He
changes all seven (+J3) -ee- spellings in copy to
-ea- and preserves twenty (+J4) of the twenty-one (+J4)
-ea- forms found in copy.
27. young
Alice Walker assigns the yong spellings to B and
young to A. The evidence in the Paviers shows that B's
treatment of young does not display a marked preference for
either form. Of the thirty-six (+J18) -ou- spellings in copy,
B
changes eight (+J6) to -o-. On the other hand, he changes
eleven (+J6) of the twenty-eight (+J8) -o- spellings to
-ou-. The number of changes of -o- to
-ou- has a slight numerical superiority over -ou-
to
-o-, but no clear-cut habit can be observed.
It is worth noting the extent to which previous investigators dealing
with Compositor B in the Folio and working without the ability to
distinguish E from B as well as without reference to copy have determined
B's preferential spellings as they are revealed in the Paviers. Four of the
five test words proposed by Willoughby as means of identifying B are in
fact strong B preferences.[21]
Twenty-one of the forms proposed by Alice Walker as characteristic B
spellings have been studied in the Paviers, and seventeen of the twenty-one
are strong preferential spellings there. Two conclusions can be drawn from
these results. First, the limited amount of text set by E and the closeness of
his overall spelling pattern to that of B have not obscured B's spellings
from previous investigators. Second, the fact that seventeen of B's Folio
habits are confirmed in the Paviers indicates the extent to which set habits
remained fixed. Although it will be necessary for
later investigations to analyze B's Folio spellings fully, the preliminary
indications are that the Paviers give an excellent idea of B's spellings. This
is not to say, however, that any one individual B spelling as observed in the
Paviers can be assumed to be his fixed preferential spelling. The general
texture of B's spellings in the Paviers, however, should be reflected in his
work in the Folio.
It is also important to note that the various words here discussed
display all of the various logical possibilities for a compositor's treatment
of variant spellings. B is seen to display strong preferences for one, two,
and even three variant spellings of given words and can display indifference
which appears neither to be influenced by a weak preference nor to depend
on copy forms.
IV.
My examination of the Pavier spellings was undertaken as a result of
the hypothesis that an analysis of compositorial spellings can provide not
merely the basis for accurately identifying a compositor's work by his
preferential spellings, but also the means of seeing through the workman to
his copy. In many situations, it would be extremely advantageous, once the
compositor has been identified, to penetrate the layer of his spellings and
go behind the man to the copy from which he was setting his type.
Compositorial identification has concentrated on the discovery of forms like
do, go, and heere which are signs
indicating that the text in question is B's work. The hypothesis in question
stated, however, that if a new group of spellings can be discovered which
contains not only these compositorial habits and preferences, but also
spellings which reproduce copy spellings as well, there will be a means of
seeing through that compositor to his copy. The
examination of the Pavier spellings has produced the evidence to verify this
hypothesis and has further shown that even the identifying badges, strong
preferences, can be used to get at copy spellings.
There are various types of words which can produce information
about copy. The ideal word is one that has two or more variant spellings
which occur in mixed groups and are mechanically reproduced by the
compositor in question. Such words which then directly depict the copy
spelling at every occurrence turn out to be extremely rare. The only
examples which are invariably reliable in the Paviers are
art(20)/arte(2),
brier(2)/bryer(2),
marvell(4)/marveile(5+J1), and
pallace(4)/pallas(1). All four of these words
occur
in the Paviers with limited frequency, but they serve to demonstrate that
words can be found which will vary in direct relation to copy.
Oh is the best example of a high frequency word which varies
in extremely close relation to copy spelling. Of the 361 mixed
O (244+J45) and Oh (177+J20) forms in B's
copy,
341 reflect copy spellings. When one encounters an O or
Oh spelling in B's
Pavier texts, therefore, the odds are better than fifteen to one that B's
spelling is the spelling which stood in copy. Words of this type which can
be said to express compositorial indifference are unfortunately relatively
scarce and are, therefore, only one contributing factor to the search for
spellings which reflect copy. When they do occur, however, the evidential
value is extremely strong.
At the other end of the scale are strong habits and preferences which,
if viewed from a new perspective and not as identifying badges indicating
a given compositor's presence, can contribute clues to copy spellings. Take,
for example, do, the strongest of B's preferential spellings.
In
all of the Paviers, B violates his strong preference fifteen times
by setting unjustified
doe spellings. Twelve of these fifteen
spellings reflect copy spellings. When a
doe form is
encountered
in B's work in contradiction to his known preference, therefore, the odds
are four to one that the
doe reflects a
doe in
copy.
The same situation holds for
goe and
here. The
odds
are fifteen to one in the former case and about thirteen to one in the latter
that these contradictions of B's preferred spellings had their origin in copy
spellings.
Between the words expressing compositorial indifference and those
which display a strong preference are all shades of a spectrum. Among
these words are many which can be termed weak preference
words. These words are those for which B displays some favorite
spelling or spellings but neither a very strong habit nor indifference.
Dear is an ideal example of a weak preference word. B
clearly
displays a preference for the deere spelling by following
every
one of the thirty-seven (+J7) deere spellings in copy and
changing twenty-three (+J6) of the sixty (+J12) deare
spellings
in copy to deere. It is the thirty-seven (+J6) non-preferential
deare spellings which B reproduces in his texts from his
copy,
however, which are of importance for copy study. B's preference was
weak, and he allowed it to be subordinated to copy spellings in these
numerous instances so that all of these deare spellings in his
texts directly reflect copy
spellings. We are, so to speak, able to see through B back to copy when we
see a deare in his text. Dear is spoken of as an
ideal
example of a weak preference, copy-reflecting word because the complete
absence of aberrant -ee- to -ea- changes makes
the
deare absolutely reliable as an indication of the copy spelling.
Every single one of the thirty-seven (+J6) -ea- forms in B's
texts directly reflects an -ea- form in copy.
There are, of course, many gradations of preferences ranging from
words that approach the strong habits like do,
go,
and heere to those which fade into examples of compositorial
indifference. In addition, many of these weak preference words are not like
deare in being 100% reliable. Perfect reliability is not,
however, a requisite for words to use in copy study. Take, for example, the
weak preference word credit. B's demonstrable preference is
for
the credite spelling, but he sets credit seven
(+J1)
times. Of these seven times, six reflect six of the thirteen
credit
spellings in copy and one represents an aberrant change of the supposedly
preferred credite to credit. This one aberrancy
does
not destroy the value of the word for the purpose of getting at copy since
the odds are still six to one that credit in the Pavier text will
reflect credit in copy.
The following compilation includes over 100 words which, within the
Pavier texts, demonstrably reflect the characteristics of copy. Here,

therefore, is the verification of the hypothesis that a class of copy-reflecting
words does exist. In the table, the second column lists B's observable
preference or preferences, if any, for a given spelling of the word in
question. The third column lists the forms which are members of the newly
discovered class of copy-reflecting spellings. The numbers in parentheses
after both forms give the number of times that these forms occur in B's
work in the Paviers. A comparison of these figures will indicate roughly the
place of that word in the spectum ranging from strong preference to
indifference. The final column is a rough attempt to indicate the reliability
of the form in column three as a reflection of copy. This percentage
indicates the fraction of the instances recorded in column three which
reflect copy. It must be noted that many of the copy-reflecting forms occur
only infrequently, but the fact that a given copy-reflecting form occurs only
once or twice does not by itself
lessen the value of that form as an indication of copy. The total picture for
each word must be examined. For example,
alreadie occurs
only
once as a copy-reflecting form but this one occurrence is highly reliable
when it is seen that B's preference for
already is relatively
strong and, therefore, any deviation from it is likely to be done under the
influence of copy. B changed nine of the ten
-ie forms in
copy
to
-y and retained all three
-y forms in copy.
The
one
alreadie, therefore, that slipped through is, like the few
doe,
goe, and
here forms which
slipped
through in contradiction of strong preferences, highly reliable as an
indication of the copy spelling.
Words have been included in this list down to a 67% degree of
reliability. This 67% figure in itself may not sound as strong as when it is
expressed in the form of two-to-one odds that the copy-reflecting spelling
does indicate the spelling in copy. This percentage is calculated only for the
unjustified occurrences of the spellings in the third column so as to remove
any possible contamination from the exigencies of justification. Since it is
impossible to draw lines dividing the spectrum of copy-reflecting words into
different groups, the percentage of reliability must be considered in
conjunction with the number of times the preferential (column two) and
copy-reflecting (column three) spellings occur in B's texts in order to
evaluate the significance of a specific spelling as a form which reflects
copy. Because of the dangers inherent in dealing with statistically small
samples, even this rough indication of relative percentages of reliability has
been omitted for words which do
not occur at least ten times in the Paviers.

Word
|
B's Preference(s) |
Spellings Reflecting Copy
|
aboard |
aboard (5) |
aboord (4+J1) |
100% |
act |
acte (12+J3) |
act (4) |
100% |
adieu |
final -u (10) |
final -w (2+J2) |
100% |
aid |
medial -i- (12+J1) |
medial -y- (3) |
100% |
alack |
alacke (18) |
alack (4) |
75% |
already |
already (12) |
alreadie (1) |
100% |
always |
alwayes (7+J1) |
alwaies (5+J1) |
100% |
and |
and (General Rule) |
& (4+J4) |
100% |
angel |
angel (5+J1) |
angell (5) |
100% |
answer |
answer (43+J7) |
final -re (12+J3) |
83% |
approach |
approch (6+J1) |
approach (5) |
100% |
art |
_____ |
art (20) |
100% |
|
|
arte (2) |
100% |
bar |
barre (9) |
bar (1) |
100% |
battle |
battell (12+J4) |
battle (1) |
100% |
|
|
battaile (7+J1) |
86% |
been |
_____ |
bin (18+J10) |
72% |
behind |
behinde (17+J2) |
behind (4+J3) |
75% |
bishop |
byshop (19) |
bishop (9) |
100% |
blind |
blinde (14+J5) |
blind (2+J2) |
100% |
blood |
blood (97+J8) |
bloud (32) |
84% |
bloody |
bloody (27+J1) |
medial -ou- (13) |
100% |
bow |
bow (4) |
bowe (1) |
box |
box (1) |
boxe (2+J1) |
briar |
_____ |
bryer (2) |
|
|
brier (2) |
brief |
breefe (5) |
briefe (12+J3) |
100% |
charity |
charity (12+J3) |
charitie (1) |
100% |
cheer |
cheere (10+J2) |
cheare (3) |
100% |
chief |
cheefe (8+J1) |
chiefe (1+J2) |
100% |
choice |
choise (21+J1) |
internal -y- (4) |
100% |
choose |
choose (5) |
chuse (3) |
clear |
cleere (9) |
cleare (11+J1) |
82% |
coin |
coine (4) |
coyne (2) |
command |
command (17+J2) |
commaund (1) |
100% |
courtesy |
courtesie (6+J1) |
curtesie (7+J1) |
100% |
cousin |
_____ |
internal -ou- (9) |
89% |
|
|
internal -o- (11+J4) |
90% |
credit |
credite (8+J3) |
credit (7+J1) |
86% |
crown |
crowne (21+J3) |
crown (1+J1) |
100% |
cruel |
cruell (13+J1) |
cruel (1) |
100% |
dear |
deere (60+J13) |
deare (37+J6) |
100% |
deed |
deede (8+J2) |
deed (14+J1) |
71% |
deny |
deny (13+J3) |
denie (1) |
100% |
devil |
divell (44+J15) |
devill (1+J1) |
100% |
do |
do (573+J103) |
doe (15+J21) |
80% |
duchess |
final -esse (9+J3) |
final -es (1+J1) |
100% |
duty |
duty (14+J1) |
dutie (1+J1) |
100% |
enemy |
enemy (12+J1) |
enemie (3) |
100% |
error |
error (7+J1) |
errour (1) |
extreme |
extreme (12) |
extreame (2) |
100% |
fancy |
fancy (4) |
fancie (1) |
forth |
foorth (25+J3) |
forth (16+J2) |
87% |
fury |
fury (9) |
furie (3) |
100% |

Word
|
B's Preference(s) |
Spellings Reflecting Copy
|
girl |
gyrle (6) |
girle (1+J2) |
go |
go (231+J40) |
goe (73+J32) |
93% |
grief |
greefe (19+J1) |
griefe (21+J4) |
100% |
grieve |
greeve (19) |
grieve (4+J3) |
75% |
guard |
guard (13+J1) |
gard (3) |
100% |
guess |
guesse (5) |
gesse (2) |
hear |
heare (19+J2) |
hear (1) |
100% |
heart |
heart (184+J27) |
hart (20+J10) |
95% |
herald |
herald (7) |
herauld (5) |
80% |
here |
heere (345+J44) |
here (150+J41) |
91% |
heretic |
hereticke (2) |
heretike (6) |
high |
high (7+J2) |
hi (y) e (9) |
100% |
hour |
houre (45+J6) |
internal -ow- (2) |
100% |
intreat |
initial en- (17+J2) |
initial in- (13+J1) |
92% |
|
final -te (22+J2) |
final -t (4+J1) |
100% |
jest |
jeast (4) |
jest (3) |
keys |
keyes (3) |
keies (1) |
kiss |
kisse (15) |
kis (2) |
100% |
liberty |
liberty (4) |
libertie (1) |
loud |
_____ |
lowd (4) |
75% |
|
|
loud (7) |
86% |
mad |
mad (13+J2) |
madde (1+J1) |
100% |
madam |
medial -d- (78+J12) |
medial -dd- (1+J1) |
100% |
maid |
internal -i- (25+J7) |
internal -y- (2) |
100% |
majesty |
majesty (47+J3) |
majestie (2+J1) |
100% |
marvel |
_____ |
marvell (4) |
100% |
|
|
marvaile (5+J1) |
100% |
master |
master (121+J19) |
maister (4+J3) |
100% |
mighty |
mighty (15+J3) |
mightie (1) |
100% |
mind |
minde (61+J10) |
mind (4+J3) |
100% |
mock |
mocke (9) |
mock (1) |
100% |
month |
month (6+J1) |
moneth (2) |
moral |
morall (4) |
morrall (1) |
murder |
_____ |
internal -d- (39+J3) |
80% |
|
|
internal -th- (34+J3) |
79% |
near |
neere (45+J4) |
neare (2) |
100% |
need |
neede (9+J2) |
need (13+J4) |
69% |
noise |
noise (5+J4) |
noyse (3) |
100% |
oh |
_____ |
o (244+J45) |
99% |
|
|
oh (117+J20) |
85% |
old |
olde (21+J18) |
final -d (42+J16) |
93% |
palace |
palace (3+J2) |
pallace (4) |
100% |
|
|
pallas (1) |
100% |
pity |
internal -tt- (42+J7) |
internal -t- (2+J1) |
100% |
poison |
poyson (8) |
poison (3) |
67% |
quick |
quicke (6+J4) |
quick (2) |
100% |
relieve |
internal -ee- (11) |
internal -ie- (1) |
100% |
run |
run (16+J2) |
runne (13+J7) |
100% |
sail |
internal -y- (10+J2) |
internal -i- (4+J1) |
100% |
seven |
seven (9) |
seaven (2) |
100% |
show(s) |
medial -e- (75+J8) |
medial -o- (13) |
100% |
singular: |
final -w (60+J3) |
final -we (2+J1) |
100% |
son |
sonne (87+J14) |
son (5+J3) |
80% |
sorry |
internal -rr- (5) |
internal -r- (3+J1) |
Word
|
B's Preference(s) |
Spellings Reflecting Copy
|
sudden |
_____ |
internal -u- (4+J2) |
100% |
|
|
internal -o- (11+J3) |
90% |
summer |
summer (6+J2) |
sommer (7+J1) |
100% |
sun |
sunne (23+J1) |
sun (5) |
80% |
together |
together (12+J2) |
togither (3) |
100% |
traitor |
internal -i- (59+J4) |
internal -y- (1+J1) |
100% |
truly |
truely (7+J2) |
truly (2+J1) |
100% |
try |
try (6+J2) |
trie (2) |
100% |
unless |
unlesse (18) |
unles (1) |
100% |
victory |
victory (6) |
victorie (2) |
war |
single r (5) |
double rr (22) |
100% |
wax |
wax (4+J2) |
waxe (2+J5) |
100% |
we'll |
wee'l (42+J5) |
final -le (10+J3) |
100% |
wind |
winde (34+J10) |
wind (3) |
100% |
witness |
witnesse (15+J4) |
witnes (1) |
100% |
you'll |
you'l (23+J3) |
youle (3) |
67% |
young |
_____ |
internal -o- (25+J8) |
68% |
|
|
internal -ou- (39+J18) |
72% |
The forms in this table which are reflections of copy constitute the
class of words hypothesized to exist at the beginning of this study. The
large number of spellings which belong to this class and the consistently
high degree of their reliability at reflecting copy spellings show that words
which reflect copy are an important part of the overall pattern of B's
spellings. Compositor B has more than simple preferential spellings; he has
a complex pattern of strong preferences, weak preferences, tolerances, and
even mechanical indifference. Time after time under the pressure of copy,
he sets non-preferential spellings about which he was relatively indifferent
and thereby allows his texts to transmit the evidence of spellings which lie
behind them. No longer must the person investigating a B text look only for
do and go and heere spellings.
He will
look for doe, goe, here,
youle, wind and so on, which by long odds are
indications of copy spellings. Using a large number of the forms which
reflect copy spellings, we can now start to assemble from the evidence in
B's texts a projection of the texture of copy spellings.
Until the Pavier statistics are carefully extended by word counts on
control situations in the Folio, no thoroughgoing application of this new
method for determining spelling features of unknown or disputed copy can
be attempted. As a brief test of the validity of this theoretical technique,
however, let us turn to the first text occurring in the 1623 Folio which was
set by B from known copy and perform a formal experiment in hopes of
indicating the validity of the technique. Such an experiment can offer a
formal demonstration under laboratory-like conditions of this new method
for determining the characteristics of
copy. From the Folio text, we can use the group of spellings reflecting
copy to predict the spellings of words in the copy used for the Folio.
Turning then to this copy, the validity or invalidity of the procedure will be
apparent.
According to Hinman's study, Compositor B set the last six pages of
Love's Labors Lost in the Folio
(M4r-M6v).[22] Examining these six Folio pages,
we find
the following forms which occur in unjustified lines and should, on the
basis of the results of the Pavier analysis, indicate copy spellings.
- alwaies 1
- bin 1
- deare 3
- griefe 1
- hart 3
- jest 5
- O 12
- show 3
- sodaine 1
- yong 1
These six Folio pages include, therefore, thirty-one spellings which, at odds
ranging from two to one (
yong) to virtual certainty
(
deare) should be reflections of spellings which stood in B's
copy.
The experimental demonstration now depends on the confirmation or
denial of the existence of these predicted phenomena by checking the
control text, Love's Labors Lost Q1 (1598).[23] Checking these predicted spellings
against
the actual copy, we find that twenty-nine of the thirty-one or 93% of the
projected spellings are in fact the spellings which stood in copy. Here is
strong evidence that the hitherto unrecognized group of copy-reflecting
spellings within B's total spelling system functions as an indication of copy.
The strength of this experiment cannot be overemphasized. Starting with an
hypothesis theoretically proposing the existence of a class of spellings which
could be used as indications of copy spellings, a class of copy-reflecting
spellings was constituted from the evidence derived from the control
situation in the Paviers. The Folio sample was then examined for the
presence of these newly discovered forms, and once they were
found, yet another control confirmed the validity of the spellings themselves
as reflections of copy and the validity of the technique of projecting copy
spellings
from the evidence in B's texts. Here is a classic case in bibliography of
hypothesis, prediction, and experimental verification.
It has now been demonstrated that a portion of a compositor's total
spelling pattern is composed of spellings which directly reflect the spellings
found in the compositor's copy. Although many investigators have
conjectures about the origins and characteristics of copy used for various
printed texts, the discovery and use of what are here called copy-reflecting
spellings is the first procedure for determining features of copy. Many
formerly vexing problems should be resolved by a thorough implementation
of this method. After further investigation of spellings which reflect copy
in Folio texts set by B from known copy, the road will be open to solve
disputed problems like the possibility of the use of an annotated hybrid of
Q1 (1608) and Q2 (1619) as copy for Folio Lear. Although
authorities agree that the Folio Lear text was set from an
annotated quarto, there is no agreement on the exact nature of copy. Using
B's copy-reflecting spellings, one may be able to
reliably project various features of the copy from B's share of the Folio text
and then by inspection see which of the quartos matches the projection. In
the case of texts set from MSS, these spellings which depend on copy
spellings may provide the first substantial evidence concerning the specific
physical features of copy. This new method, therefore, makes a start at
enabling textual investigators to tackle one of the most vexing problems in
establishing texts of English Renaissance drama, the nature of the lost MS
copy behind extant printed texts. More specifically, when the technique has
been applied to all of the workmen in the Shakespeare Folio, we may then
possess an excellent tool for getting closer to the copy behind the texts of
important Shakespearean plays that were printed for the first time in the
Folio. The day may come when we can apply these techniques to texts set
from Shakespearean foul papers and arrive at a far more intimate
knowledge of the orthographical
features of Shakespearean holograph than is at all possible today.
Notes