Roberts' Compositors in Titus
Andronicus Q2
by
Paul L. Cantrell and George Walton
Williams
The problem of distinguishing the work of James Roberts' two
compositors, X and Y, in Titus Andronicus Q2 (1600)
provides
an interesting compositorial analysis. John Russell Brown first
revealed the orthographical personalities of these two men and
identified their work in two other Roberts' quartos, Hamlet
Q2 (1604/5) and The Merchant of Venice (1600).[1]
The evidence for the respective stints of the workmen in the two
latter plays is quite definite and clear-cut. Titus
Andronicus, however, offers a different problem for two main
reasons: first, because it is a fairly accurate reprint of a
previous edition; second, because the two men did not set their
respective shares of copy in well-defined and obvious
bibliographical units.
The printer's copy for Titus Andronicus Q2 was an
exemplar of the first edition printed in 1594 by John Danter.
Hamlet Q2 and The Merchant of Venice were
set
from
manuscript. It is ordinarily accepted that the identification of
different compositors is more difficult in a reprint than in an
original edition set from manuscript. In some cases it may be
impossible. The reasons for this are easy to see. Considering the
somewhat casual nature of Elizabethan manuscript copy, it is only
natural that when a compositor comes to set up this manuscript into
type, to some extent he will—almost unwittingly—impose his
own
personal orthographical and typographical characteristics upon it
in his ordering it for the press. From previous studies, it appears
that no two compositors are likely to do this in the same way.[2] The transition is a real one, a
matter
of transferring comparatively unorganized handwritten material into
organized
material suitable for printing.
The case for a reprint is quite different. Here the copy is
printed material
with an orthographical and structural order which has already been
imposed upon it by previous compositors. The normal tendency is for
the reprint compositors conveniently to follow the original print
in many matters of form, spelling, and typographical detail. In a
recent article on compositorial determination, Dr. Alice Walker
writes: "Reprints are, of course, a trickier problem than first
editions, since we must expect the trail to be confused by the
spellings of the print used as copy. Roberts's reprint of
Titus
Andronicus certainly gives a much distorted picture of the
habits of the compositors who set it and suggests that it may be
little use to try to arrive at a compositor's normal practice from
a reprint."
[3]
The problem of Titus Q2 is further complicated by the
fact that in the reprint X and Y did not combine to set their
material in a normal pattern for two-compositor work in which each
man serves a different press. In fact, the peculiar feature of
Titus is that there should be a second compositor at all.
The running-title pattern indicates no such second workman. The
book was printed throughout with one skeleton-forme, and so
necessarily on one press, with the one set of four running-titles
transferred from inner forme to outer forme in orderly succession.
The only aberration occurs in sheet K where the recto quarters were
exchanged diagonally in the forme as they were shifted from outer
I to inner K, and then are maintained in that reversed position
into outer K. There is nothing in this minor shift, however, that
has anything to do with the question of a second compositor. And
this second compositor, Roberts' X, is at work long before K.
In Hamlet and The Merchant, each
compositor's
work-unit was the sheet, with the sole exception of sheet L in
Hamlet, where X set two pages, L1r and
L4v, and Y
the
remainder, L1v-L4r. In
Hamlet, X set sheets B-D, F,
I,
N, and O + A; Y set sheets E, G, H, M, K, and L (with the exception
of two pages as noted above[4] ). In
The Merchant, Y set sheets
A, B, D, F, and H; X set sheets C, E, G, I, and K. Thus in these
two plays an equable pattern of composing is observed which is not
to be seen in
Titus, where Compositor Y seems to have been
responsible for the bulk of the reprint, but we find X assisting
him at unpredictable intervals and for differing lengths of time.
Such irregularity, along with the fact that
Titus is a
reprint, makes it harder to distinguish the work-pattern of the
compositors.
Mr. Brown's work on Titus Andronicus naturally
stemmed
from the data for his successful spelling tests in Hamlet
and The Merchant, but many of the significant factors did
not apply since Titus was a reprint. Of the twenty-odd
paired spellings for X and Y which he found in Hamlet and
The Merchant,[5] only
nine
have any significance for Titus, and these in varying
degrees. Those that he did find, however, are meaningful because
they definitely identify X and Y as being the same two compositors
in Roberts' shop.
On the strength of all of the evidence he could garner which
separated X and Y in Hamlet and The
Merchant,
Mr.
Brown tentatively assigned 38 of the 79 pages of type
(A1v is
blank) in Titus to Y, and 14 pages to X, leaving 27 pages
unassigned. His allocation is as follows:
- Y: A1, 2, 3v, 4, 4v,
B2v, 4, C1, 1v, 2, 3v,
D1, 1v, 2, 4, E1, 1v,
2v, 3v, 4, F1, 1v, 2,
3v,
4, 4v, G1v, 2, 4v, H2,
3, I1, 1v, 4v, K1, 1v,
2,
2v.
- X: B1, 1v, 2, 4v,
C4v, D2v, 3, 3v, E3,
4v,
F3, I2v, 3, 3v.
It is the purpose of this present paper to indicate the results
of another examination of the compositorial problem in
Titus, an examination which will amend and augment Mr.
Brown's findings and which will allow of a new and more certain
allocation of pages. This further examination confirms how many of
the characteristics of Hamlet and The Merchant
are
not applicable to Titus, owing to the retention of many
uncharacteristic forms by the two compositors in the reprint.
Re-examination also indicates that at least four of the spelling
factors which Mr. Brown used for tentative
attribution to X and Y in
Titus are without supported
relevance and must be omitted from the analysis. These are the
-ow,-ew/-owe,-ewe distinctions, and the spellings
honor, etc./
honour, etc.,
sweete/sweet,
maddam(
e)/madam (
e), which Mr.
Brown
notes are
"not entirely trustworthy" (p. 34).
This leaves him a small core of nine very strong differentiating
factors. These invariable factors, which we may designate as Class
I of the evidence, are the more significant because their Q2 form
in Titus is a change from that in the Q1 copy. These words
and the pages on which they occur in Q2 are:
|
Q1
|
Q2
|
Q2 Pages
|
(Y-FORMS) |
their |
theyr |
A1, A2, A4(3), A4v, D1(2), E1(2),
E1v, F1v(2), F2,
F4v, G1v(2), I1, I1v(3),
I4v(3), K1, K1v(3),
K2v. |
|
mooued |
moued |
F1v
|
|
deare, -er |
deere, -er |
A4(2), C1, D2, E2v, E3v(2), E4,
F1, F1v, I1v,
K1,
K2v. |
|
being |
beeing |
D1v
|
|
ile |
Ile |
I1 |
|
choise |
choyse |
F3v
|
|
noise |
noyse[6]
|
C4 |
(X-FORMS) |
Ile |
ile |
D3, E3, E4v, I3(2), I3v
|
|
moude, proue |
moou'd, prooue |
B4v, D2v
|
|
perceiue, deceiude |
perceaue, deceau'd |
E3, I2v
|
|
howres |
houres |
C4v
|
Two facts may be instantly noted from this array: seven of Y's
pages and one for X are confirmed by the appearance of two factors,
and no pages contain conflicting X and Y evidence.
Further, since the Ile/ile distinction is so strong in
The Merchant of Venice (printed the same year as
Titus), we may with some confidence admit as oblique but
suggestive evidence those instances where Ile is copied from
Q1 to Q2 as being Y's normal practice; similarly, the one instance
where ile is copied from Q1 to Q2 we may attribute to X.
This attribution allows further confirmation of seven pages already
allocated to Y, tentatively adds four new pages for him, and adds
one new page for X. These are the occurrences of the straight
copying:[7]
- Y: Ile B2v(2), D1v, D4,
E2v(2), F1(3), F4v(3),
G3v, H2v, I4v, K1,
K1v.
- X: ile H4v.
Still no conflicts occur. This fact, along with the fact that
seven previously-established pages were confirmed for Y, is almost
the sole justification for the admission of such evidence. Proof of
this sort is tenuous and inferential at best, for it may very well
be that the compositor was faithfully adhering to his printed copy.
This faithfulness is a practice which X, unfortunately, appears all
too prone to follow. The point is that in analyzing a reprint,
inferential evidence of a negative character is sometimes the only
evidence available because of the nature of the work. It should
only be used, ideally, when at least three of the following four
conditions are met: (1) when no conflicts develop; (2) when
confirmations for previously-allocated pages are obtained;[8] (3) when the factor is a very
strong
one, verified by reference to other works set by the same man; (4)
when there is an almost invariable positive factor (a definite
change of the
same word in the copy-spelling) by the other compositor to balance
the permissive, negative factor. Of such a nature is X's spelling
of deare, and Y's spelling of howrely and
here, which will be discussed later.
The re-application of a spelling test to the three Roberts'
plays also uncovered several pieces of fresh evidence which fell
into three general categories:
- (1) New, differentiating characteristics found in a
reappraisal of Hamlet and The Merchant,
unnoted
by
Brown, which also have significance for Titus.
- (2) Identifying factors, very few in number, which serve to
differentiate the two men in Titus, but which do not appear
in either Hamlet or The Merchant.
- (3) Pertinent copy-spellings which are retained from Q1, but
which, under the limitations just discussed above, it seems safe to
admit as permissive factors of negative evidence.
These three categories of fresh evidence we may designate Class
II. Obviously, these categories are not of equal weight, and are
valuable in a descending scale of importance. The last two are
considerably weaker than the first, which, because not invariable,
is itself weaker than those invariable factors in Class I with
which we began.
In the first category of Class II—those factors which are
applicable to all three plays—five more determining
characteristics for Y were uncovered; none, regrettably, for X.
These five are: (1) Y's preference for the ampersand, which he uses
about three times as much as X, a practice particularly revealing
in The Merchant;[9] (2)
Y's
exclusive use of the tilde,
usually for justification;
[10] (3) his
preference for the
oo-form in
doone, dooing,
and
in
doost;
[11] (4) his
occasional
spelling of
do;
[12] (5)
and
his
spelling of
greefe(
-s, -ues).
[13]
The following assignments in Titus are predicated on
these fresh distinctions:
(Y-FORMS) |
Q1
|
Q2
|
Q2 Pages
|
|
and |
&[14]
|
B1v, C3v, D2, E1 |
|
done |
doone |
I1v, K2 |
|
doing |
dooing |
I1 |
|
dost |
doost |
C2v, D3v, D4,
E1v, F2, G2(2), I4v
|
|
doe |
do |
F3, I4v
|
|
them |
thē |
I4v
|
|
griefe, etc. |
greefe, etc. |
C1(2), E4, I2 |
In addition to confirming ten Y-pages, this last group of spellings
common to all three plays has added eight new pages to Y's span,
B1
v, C2
v, C3
v,
D3
v, F3, G2, I2, and K2, pages which
had
not as yet been assigned.
In the second category of new evidence, four identifying factors
unique to Titus were found, one of which serves to separate
X and Y, but the other three to add to our pages for Y. It appears
certain that Compositor Y altered the Q1 speech prefix Moore
to Aron in Q2 on C2v, D1, F1, and
G1v. In a
typographical change, he altered the alignment of stage directions
on E1, where a two-line reverse pyramid is converted to one in
which the lines are equally indented, and on C4, G3v, K1,
and
K1v, where paragraphed stage directions are identically
altered
to this indented form. Sig. K1v offers a typical example:
- Q1: Sound Trumpets. Enter Emperour and Empresse with
Tribunes and others.
- Q2: Sound Trumpets. Enter Emperour and Empresse with
Tribunes and others.
It is also fairly certain, as Brown assumed, that Compositor Y
changed the Q1 spelling of
this to
thys on
C1
v(2),
C2(2), C3
v, F4(2), and I1
v. In addition,
pages for both X and
Y may be allocated by their differing spelling, both in the text
and in the speech prefixes, of the name of Saturninus' brother, as
indicated by the following divergences from the copy:
|
Q1
|
Q2
|
Q2 Pages
|
Y: |
Bascianus |
Bassianus[15]
|
B2v(2), C1, D1, D1v
|
X: |
Bassianus |
Bascianus |
A3, B4v, D4v(6) |
All of these four practices (for they can hardly be called
characteristics on the basis of one play) have the virtue of
confirming an array of Y-pages already allocated to him on other
evidence. The three new pages which are added for him—C2, C4,
and
F4—have no X evidence in them. Conversely, of the three pages
attributed to X on the basis of his spelling of Bascianus,
one of them, B4v, is now confirmed, and two new
ones—A3 and
D4v—are added, with no conflict with any Y
evidence. In fact,
the more evidence that accumulates, the more meaningful the lack of
any Y characteristics becomes for any given page.
The last category of new evidence in Class II, three examples of
negative copying, may be cited with less certainty. The
stipulations that must be observed before this sort of evidence can
be admitted have already been set out above. The spellings
are:[16]
|
Q1
|
Q2
|
Q2 Pages
|
Y: |
here |
here |
A4v(5), B1v,
D1v, D2(3), E1, E3v,
F1v(2),
F2,
F3v(2), G1v(2), G1(2), G2(2),
G4v(3), H1v(3), H2,
K1. |
|
howrely |
howrely |
G2v
|
X: |
deare(-er, -ly) |
deare(-er, -ly)[17]
|
B4, F2v, F3, H4. |
Compositor Y's copying of here in Q2 as it is found in
Q1
is a case in point for permissive evidence. It is again
inferential, but the attribution is formulated on sound support.
Compositor X is almost invariably a heere-speller;
only twice (once for obvious justification) does he use the simpler
spelling in
The Merchant (G2,K1), and at no time in
Hamlet. Y uses the short form nine times in
The
Merchant (A2
v, D2(2), H1(2), H2(2),
H2
v, H3
v), and
twice in
Hamlet (M1
v(2)). Of the 31 times
this short
spelling is used in
Titus, on no occasion does a conflict
develop with X evidence.
[18] One
copy-spelling of
deare conflicts with Y's spelling of
do on F3 (see below).
Finally, in Class III, there are two other rather general
typographical practices which seem indicative of differing
treatment by the two men, not always distinctive, but more
correlative than not. Each practice involves a choice on the part
of the compositors: the first is the placement of one sort of stage
direction; the second is the spelling of the speech prefixes in the
transfer from the original to the reprint.
In the matter of certain stage directions, the point of
difference is their comparative centering in the line, whether it
is precise or only approximate. Excluded from consideration are
those stage directions which are not amenable to centering (or
which appeared so to X and Y)—those either obviously flush right
or nearly right in Q1 (such as Exit, Exeunt, etc.), or those
(like the indented forms already noted as set by Y) which, whatever
their indentation or paragraphing, continue to the right margin, or
nearly so, and take up the bulk of the line. Within a tolerance of
two millimeters, Compositor X as a general proposition made sure
that his medial stage directions were precisely centered in the
line, or to put it another way, were centered in his long stick.
This involved setting the stage direction first, then placing quads
or blank spaces equally on either side of the setting until the
line was of the proper length. Compositor Y, however, while he
obviously intended
these stage directions to be approximately in the middle of the
line, made no such precise effort to center them, and apparently
guessed at the spacing on either side; hence, his medial stage
directions usually deviate from the midpoint of the line in varying
degrees. Into his stick, which was the same length as X's, Y would
evidently put first the amount of quads or spaces which he felt
would place the stage direction in the approximate center of the
line, set the stage direction, then fill the line out with the
number of spaces required for justification. This procedure most
often yielded an off-center setting, but, depending on the accuracy
of the guess, sometimes accidentally resulted in a centered
setting.
The 22 pages in which these precisely-centered stage directions
occur may be divided as follows: (1) nine pages where X's presence
is confirmed: A3, B4v, C4v,
D4v, E3, E4v, H1, H4, and
I2v; (2) six hitherto neutral pages on which no prior
evidence
has been established either way: A3v, B3,
B3v, E2, H3v,
and I4; (3) seven pages attributed to Y with which this evidence
conflicts: A2v, D3v, D4,
F1v, F3, G4v, and
H1v.
Non-centered stage directions which we have postulated as the
result of Y's practice occur on no X-pages, a fact certainly
meaningful, and thus serve to confirm the following 13 pages
already suggested for Y: A4, A4v, C2, C4, D1, D2,
E1v,
E2v, F1, H2, H2v, I2, and K2.
While not always decisive, the above evidence does show a high
degree of correlation with what has already been established with
antecedent material.[19]
In the matter of speech prefixes, from what is observable in the
pages already assigned to the two men it would appear that X's
general tendency is to copy the speech prefix exactly as it stands
in Q1; on the other hand, it seems to have been Y's general
tendency to alter the original settings several times on a page,
more often by contraction than by expansion. Such a broad division
tends to support many pages previously allocated. This general
correlation can be seen by examining what happened to the speech
prefixes on the pages which, either on strong evidence or weak,
have been already allocated in this study. In the following table,
the numerator of the fraction represents the number of changes made
in the speech prefixes, the denominator the number of speech
prefixes on the page, and therefore the number of opportunities for
change of copy.[20]
- (X-PAGES)[21] B4 0/13;
B4v
0/8, C4v 0/9; D2v 2/12; D3 0/10;
D4v 0/9; E3 0/8; E4v
0/7; F2v 0/1; H1 0/12; H4 0/3; H4v 2/9;
I2v 0/6; I3 1/7;
I3v 2/7; K3v 0/4; K4 0/5.
- (Y-PAGES) A4 2/7; A4v 0/3;
B1v 3/11; B2v 4/18;
C1
1/4; C1v 3/7; C2 1/3; C2v 5/9;
C3v 3/9; C4 0/4; D1 1/2;
D1v 1/7; D2 3/7; D3v 2/10; D4 3/8; E1
5/13; E1v 3/3;
E2v 0/4; E3v 0/5; E4 2/7; F1 1/10;
F1v 1/6; F2 2/8; F3
3/7; F3v 4/11; F4 2/4; F4v 0/7; G1 3/8;
G1v 7/14; G2 6/20;
G2v 2/8; G3v 2/4; G4v
0/6; H1v 1/7; H2 0/2; H2v
2/9;
I1 2/8; I2 2/9; I4v 2/15; K1 0/1; K1v
2/6; K2 2/13; K2v
2/11.
Analysis of this array is tabulated as follows:
Compositor |
no. of pages |
no. of pages on which changes occur |
no. of pages on which no changes occur |
total no. of prefix changes |
total no. of prefixes |
% of change |
X |
17 |
4 |
13 |
7 |
130 |
5.3 |
Y |
43 |
35 |
8 |
90 |
335 |
26.8 |
There is no doubt that the difference in the percentage of change
is statistically valid.
Lack of speech-prefix change is the only evidence appearing on
six pages, B1, C3[22], G3, G4, H3,
and
K3, where 4, 7, 9, 5, 6, and 1 speech prefixes, respectively, are
unchanged; but since the practice is not invariable, any
attribution to X must still be indecisive. On the other hand, four
pages contain only speech-prefix evidence combined with
comparative-centering evidence: A3v,
B3v, E3, and H3v,
where both practices combine to suggest X.[23] Two pages contain no other
evidence
except that of speech-prefix changes: B2 3/5; and K4v
1/2.
The final allocation of pages in Titus is therefore
predicated on material of various degrees of assurance. In Class I
are placed those pages for which there is strong evidence:[24]
- Y: A1, A2, A4, A4v, C1, C4, D1,
D1v, D2, E1,
E1v,
E2v, E3v, E4, F1, F1v,
F2, F3v, F4v, G1v, I1,
I1v, I4v, K1, K1v,
K2v.
- X: B4v, C4v,
D2v, D3, E3, E4v, I2v,
I3,
I3v.
In Class II are those assignments for which there is fresh evidence
based on similar occurrences in
Hamlet and
The
Merchant:
- Y: B1v, C2v,
C3v, D3v, D4, F3, G2, I2,
K2;
for which there is fresh evidence unique for
Titus:
- Y: B2v, C1v, C2, F4,
G3v
- X: A3, D4v;
and for which there is only negative permissive evidence:
- Y: A2v, G1, G2v,
G4v, H1v, H2,
H2v
- X: B4, F2v, H1, H4, H4v,
K3v, K4.
In Class III are those assignments for which the evidence is purely
tentative, based on a correlation of speech-prefix and
comparative-centering evidence only:
If we may bring together all ranges of evidence, the pattern of
assignment is:
There remains only briefly to consider why Titus was
composed in such an irregular pattern, or why Compositor X came to
assist on the book at all. One hypothesis, consonant with what we
know went on in Elizabethan printing shops, would in general
explain X's irregular share of the work and also account for the
fact that most of the X-pages occur late in the sheet. This
hypothesis, based on the assumption that Titus was composed
in continuous but not simultaneous setting by the two men, is
simply that X was called in to help while Y was distributing the
type from a forme that already had been printed. We know that
distribution was a necessary and regular operation throughout the
printing of a book. If, at the times Y was distributing, X were
available, or could easily be made available, this postulation
could be feasible. Since, theoretically, Y would be ready for
distribution at about the same time in relation to the composing
schedule of each sheet—distribution
which would not of course take exactly the same amount of time for
each forme—Compositor X could then be called in approximately at
the same time in the schedule for each sheet. While Y was
distributing, it is reasonable to suppose that X continued with the
setting of new pages. If X was not in precise control of the exact
moment when he could become available, this delay would account for
his irregular entrances into the sheets, perhaps in the middle or
lower part of a page, since, presumably, Y would leave off
composing only
when X appeared. This could easily have happened when, in a stint
of two or three consecutive X-pages, an X-factor occurs only in the
lower section of the first page of the stint. Similarly, on the
conclusion of his distribution, Y would not always return to
relieve X at the precise moment of a bibliographical break within
the forme, and could take over at some place within a page which X
had begun.
Since, as has been estimated, distribution time is about
one-third of composing time, this division of the work would
explain the shorter share of X, which on the basis of the
allocations posited in this study is about one-third that of
Y's.
In conclusion, any complete division of Titus
Andronicus
by compositors is at this moment impracticable. The larger picture
is reasonably clear, but beyond the allocation of those pages
established in this study, it is now difficult to proceed without
an undue amount of conjecture. Further progress must await two
desiderata, the accumulation of additional knowledge of the
compositorial habits of X and Y by examination of other works,
preferably plays, set by these two men, either together or singly;
and subsequent research from Titus itself, or
Hamlet,
or The Merchant of Venice, which will reveal significant
materials unnoticed so far.
The present results are not completely inutile: enough definite
pages have been noted for the two men to be of use in analyzing
what they did to printed copy, information which has its use
elsewhere.
Notes