![]() | | ![]() |

Compositor B, The Pavier Quartos, and Copy
Spellings
by
William S. Kable
Printed books are the chief textual witnesses for the majority of Renaissance English literature. Lacking authorial manuscripts, our knowledge of the words from the author's pen comes to us through the medium of print. Modern bibliographical researches have only begun to explore fully the possibilities of reducing this distance between the author's pen and the extant printed text by investigating the process by which the author's words reached print. In this beginning, however, the printing house compositor has already emerged as a significant factor in the transformation of manuscript into printed text. Compositorial study concentrates on the identification and evaluation of the work of these men, and a comprehensive spelling analysis is one of the first necessities in any thoroughgoing compositor study.
I.
In June 1920, Thomas Satchell announced the discovery of two distinct spelling patterns which indicated the presence of two identifiable compositors involved in the setting of type for Macbeth in the First Folio Shakespeare. Satchell named these compositors A and B and outlined their significant spelling patterns based on his researches in Macbeth. Compositor A was recognized principally by the spellings doe, goe, and here; Compositor B, by do, go, heere.[1]
E. E. Willoughby extended Satchell's identification of A and B to other parts of the Folio and suggested the hypothesis that "since in the portion we have investigated there are many passages that are not characteristic of either of them [A and B], it seems probable that there was also another pair of compositors at work."[2]

Alice Walker attempted a start at assessing the quality of the work of A and B with special reference to I Henry IV, but little real progress was made at refining compositorial study in the Folio until Charlton Hinman undertook his exhaustive study of the many copies of the Folio at the Folger Shakespeare Library.[3] Hinman's use of type and case identification added a new dimension to compositor study in the Folio. In 1957 he was able to announce the discovery and isolation of a fifth, apprentice, Compositor E, whose work had clouded the picture in the Tragedies.[4] Until Hinman was able to demonstrate the presence of this fifth compositor on the basis of indisputable physical evidence, the similarity of E's most important spelling habits to those of B had caused investigators to assign E's work to B. The coincidence that do, go, and heere were both B's and E's preferred spellings of these words delayed the separation of their work until Hinman's new approach was applied to the problem.
Publication of Hinman's completed study of the printing of the Folio provides the first step toward a complete investigation of all five Jaggard compositors involved in setting type for the Folio.[5] His findings in respect to the three key words for compositor identification can be summarized as follows:
- Compositor A — doe, goe, here
- Compositor B — do, go, heere
- Compositor C — doe, goe, heere
- Compositor D — doe, goe (with tolerance for do and go found in copy), here
- Compositor E — do, go, heere (with early tolerance for copy spellings)

The ten Shakespearean and pseudo-Shakespearean plays which make up the group called the Pavier quartos were stumbling blocks to bibliographical and textual researchers before the discovery and demonstration in 1908-1910 that they are all simple page-for-page reprints produced in Isaac Jaggard's shop for Thomas Pavier in 1619.[7] Several of these reprints had even been incorrectly identified as the true first editions of their texts on the basis of substantive variants. The following table lists the Pavier titles and the copy from which they were set. The printers' names are appended to the list of copy to show the diversity of copy which served for the Paviers.[8]
Pavier Texts | Greg # | Copy | Printed by: |
2 Henry VI | 119(c) | Q1(1594) | Thomas Creede |
3 Henry VI | 138(c) | O1(1595) | Peter Short |
Pericles | 284(d) | Q3(1611) | Simon Stafford |
A Yorkshire Tragedy | 272(b) | Q1(1608) | Richard Bradock |
The Merchant of Venice | 172(b) | Q1(1600) | James Roberts |
Merry Wives of Windsor | 187(b) | Q1(1602) | Thomas Creede |
King Lear | 265(b) | Q1(1608) | Nicholas Okes |
Henry V | 165(c) | Q1(1600) | Thomas Creede |
Sir John Oldcastle | 166(b) | Q1(1600) | Valentine Simmes |
Midsummer Night's Dream | 170(b) | Q1(1600) | Richard Bradock(?) |
After the initial argumentative flurry over the identification of the true nature of the Pavier quartos, Shakespearean bibliographical and textual study has in general ignored them or, at best, hastily dismissed them as simple reprints. It is true that there is little chance that the Paviers will produce any startlingly new substantive witness to the Shakespeare text, but since these ten reprints are all products of Jaggard's printing house just a few years before the production of the all-important Folio in that very shop, they offer an excellent opportunity

D. F. McKenzie pointed the way to the significance of the Pavier quartos in a preliminary study of "Compositor B's Role in The Merchant of Venice Q2 (1619)."[9] Although his spelling tests were based on the word list of Alice Walker, who in turn compiled her list before Compositor E had been distinguished from B, McKenzie's statistics capably demonstrated that Jaggard's Compositor B as recognized in the Folio had in fact set the entire Pavier quarto of The Merchant of Venice. It must be remembered that E, the only compositor who could easily be confused with B, did not enter the shop until 1622. McKenzie was then able to observe exactly what B did with his copy, Merchant Q1 (1600). His study was divided between the few substantive alterations which B gratuitously introduced into the text and a brief discussion of B's "alteration of accidentals."
McKenzie did not, however, suggest another important use which can be made of the Pavier reprints and the controls which exist for the study of these texts. In the past, compositorial identification has concentrated on the peculiarities of the workman, on the individual mark which he leaves on the copy he sets. Jaggard's Compositor B, for example, is known primarily for his strong spelling habits, do, go, and heere. Texts set by B are noted for the virtual invariability of these spellings. There is another aspect, however, to compositor study which has been neglected in the concentration on the features of a workman's habits which are his badge and serve to identify him from his fellows. After the bibliographer has determined with assurance that a given compositor did in fact set a given portion of text, the value of the identifying spellings is exhausted. A do, go, or heere in a Folio text may be of use in assigning that text to Compositor B, but its value ends there.
In many instances, it would be useful to be able to penetrate the layer of compositorial spellings and go behind the workman to the features of his copy. The study of a new class of words in relation to individual compositors may enable the textual investigator to do just that. These words are those for which the compositor's spelling treatment is recognizable but not so pronounced as to be called an invariable habit. For example, a do in a B text can represent any form of the

The Pavier quartos provide just the controlled situation necessary to make it possible to discover whether this hypothetical group of words does in fact exist within a compositor's total spelling pattern and to establish a broader picture of the nature of compositorial spelling habits, including weak preferences and the cases of relative indifference. First, however, it is necessary to identify the compositor(s) at work in the Paviers.
The following tables represent the results of a count of the do, go, here spellings throughout the Paviers.
2H6 | 3H6 | PER | YT | MV | MW | KL | H5 | SJO | MND | Total | |
Do | 45 | 47 | 76 | 15 | 101 | 55 | 98 | 48 | 89 | 102 | 676 |
Doe | 2 | 0 | 11 | 1 | 12 | 1 | 4 | 0 | 1 | 4 | 36 |
2H6 | 3H6 | PER | YT | MV | MW | KL | H5 | SJO | MND | Total | |
Go | 56 | 16 | 10 | 3 | 41 | 29 | 37 | 20 | 38 | 21 | 271 |
Goe | 5 | 3 | 11 | 5 | 16 | 23 | 21 | 1 | 3 | 17 | 105 |
2H6 | 3H6 | PER | YT | MV | MW | KL | H5 | SJO | MND | Total | |
Heere | 57 | 38 | 48 | 11 | 62 | 29 | 38 | 16 | 58 | 32 | 389 |
Here | 22 | 11 | 21 | 3 | 15 | 24 | 33 | 8 | 25 | 29 | 191 |
Although the strength of do and the more or less strong preferences for go and heere seem to point to Compositor B, the real value of these tables lies in demonstrating the dangerous invalidity of overly simplified

First, there is no recognition of the copy spellings which lie behind these statistics. For example, the evidence for do (101 occurrences) as opposed to doe (twelve occurrences) in The Merchant of Venice seems at first to conflict with Compositor B's almost invariable do habit. A closer look at this group of spellings with the additional evidence of the spellings of the copy text, however, makes an extremely strong case in favor of Compositor B's having set The Merchant. Of the twelve doe spellings in the Pavier, all twelve reproduce B's non-habitual doe as found in copy, Q1 (1600). Of the 101 do spellings in the Pavier, only five were found in copy and ninety-six represent gratuitous changes of doe to do on the part of the compositor. To state these facts yet another way, the compositor of Pavier Merchant found the word do spelled doe 108 times and do five times in his copy. He reproduced all five do spellings and changed ninety-six of the 108 doe to do leaving only twelve doe forms found in his copy. This additional evidence of the copy spellings overwhelmingly shows the strength of the do spelling in the habits of the compositor who set the Pavier Merchant even though the raw statistics point to a partial use of the doe spelling.
This distinction between the raw numerical totals in the first case and the weighted strength provided by the evidence of copy spellings is essentially similar to that between quantitative and qualitative evidence.[10] The quantities of spellings, do (101) and doe (twelve), did in fact point in the general direction of B's habit, but the quality of the evidence only becomes apparent after a review of the copy spellings reveals that ninety-six changes of doe to do are hidden in the raw statistics. Given the relative value of any one change over any one instance of following copy, the quality of the evidence makes the case for B one of certainty. Failure to consider the copy spellings greatly weakens and obscures the genuine evidence. From this example, an important principle governing the study of compositorial habits can be formulated. In compositorial analysis of texts for which the copy is known and available, all consideration of variant spellings must include an examination of the copy spellings.
The second factor which was omitted from the first group of tables is that of the influence of justification on a compositor's spellings. It

In any given line, the possible influence of justification on the spellings is a matter for debate. The possibility of such influence must, however, cause one to segregate the spelling evidence which could be contaminated by the need for justification. Take, for example, the statistics for go/goe in King Lear. The simple counts record thirty-seven go and twenty-one goe spellings. When the possible effects of justification are taken into consideration, these statistics become go thirty-one times plus six justified occurrences and goe seven times plus fourteen justified occurrences. The original ratio of thirty-seven go to twenty-one goe spellings is rather questionable evidence for B's go as a preferential spelling. When the justified and, therefore, possibly deceptive spellings are taken away, the ratio of thirty-one go to seven goe becomes much more convincing. When this allowance for justification is combined with a consideration of the copy spellings, the total strength of the evidence emerges. Go was found ten times in Lear Q1 (1608), seven times in short lines and three times in justified lines. All ten of these go spellings are retained in the Pavier Lear. On the other hand, Lear Q1 contained thirty-one goe spellings in short lines and seventeen in justified lines. Of the thirty-one long spellings, twenty-four were changed to go. Apparently, therefore, the factor of justification caused the compositor's preference for the short go spelling to be obscured in the justified occurrences of this word. A second principle to govern spelling analysis is that the possible influence of justification on variant spellings must be acknowledged, and spellings in long lines must be separated from other spellings in any statistical compilation.
The following set of tables represents a restatement of the evidence for do, go, and here in the light of these two principles. Note that the words are recorded only in relation to copy spellings and that evidence in justified lines is separated from the general statistics. In these tables, J stands for justified, or at least long line, occurrences.

Copy→ | |||||||||||
1619 | 2H6 | 3H6 | PER | YT | MV | MW | KL | H5 | SJO | MND | Totals |
Do→Do | 41 | 12 | 15 | 3 | 3 | 45 | 22 | 48 | 57 | 24 | 270 |
J4 | J6 | J4 | J2 | J10 | J5 | J15 | J5 | J51 | |||
Do→ | 2 | 1 | 3 | ||||||||
Doe | J2 | J1 | J1 | J1 | J5 | ||||||
Doe→ | 35 | 34 | 6 | 87 | 59 | 13 | 69 | 303 | |||
Do | J21 | J2 | J9 | J12 | J4 | J4 | J52 | ||||
Doe→ | 2 | 8 | 1 | 1 | 12 | ||||||
Doe | J7 | J4 | J2 | J3 | J16 | ||||||
Copy→ | |||||||||||
1619 | 2H6 | 3H6 | PER | YT | MV | MW | KL | H5 | SJO | MND | Totals |
Go→ | 49 | 13 | 3 | 15 | 7 | 18 | 20 | 4 | 129 | ||
Go | J3 | J1 | J4 | J3 | J10 | J21 | |||||
Go→ | 2 | 1 | 1 | 1 | 5 | ||||||
Goe | J1 | J3 | J2 | J6 | |||||||
Goe→ | 4 | 3 | 5 | 34 | 9 | 24 | 2 | 5 | 16 | 102 | |
Go | J4 | J17 | J1 | J3 | J3 | J1 | J19 | ||||
Goe→ | 3 | 2 | 4 | 5 | 13 | 16 | 7 | 1 | 17 | 68 | |
Goe | J6 | J2 | J3 | J14 | J1 | J26 | |||||
Copy→ | |||||||||||
1619 | 2H6 | 3H6 | PER | YT | MV | MW | KL | H5 | SJO | MND | Totals |
Heere→ | 2 | 19 | 23 | 7 | 51 | 5 | 7 | 1 | 8 | 11 | 134 |
Heere | J7 | J2 | J5 | J2 | J2 | J18 | |||||
Heere→ | 3 | 2 | 6 | 1 | 1 | 13 | |||||
Here | J1 | J1 | J1 | J3 | J2 | J1 | J9 | ||||
Here→ | 51 | 19 | 14 | 1 | 6 | 19 | 25 | 15 | 42 | 19 | 211 |
Heere | J4 | J4 | J1 | J3 | J6 | J6 | J2 | J26 | |||
Here→ | 19 | 7 | 16 | 2 | 6 | 21 | 18 | 8 | 17 | 23 | 137 |
Here | J3 | J2 | J1 | J14 | J7 | J5 | J32 |
Only in these complete tables which take the copy spellings and justification into consideration does the overwhelming evidence for Compositor B as the man who set type for the entire set of the Paviers become apparent. The already small total of thirty-six spellings in the first, overly simplified, tables becomes even more insignificant when it

The go/goe evidence was not at all clear-cut in the first table but here is shown to be significantly in favor of Compositor B's go. Of the 105 goe spellings in the Paviers, ninety-four (and of that ninety-four, twenty-six justified) reproduce copy spellings. There are only five non-justified, non-copy goe spellings while there are 101 similar go forms.
The here/heere ratio was also not clear-cut in the simplified table. With the additional factors considered, however, 169 of the 191 here spellings turn out to derive from the copy used for the Paviers. Of that 169, thirty-two were in justified lines. The thirteen non-copy, non-justified here spellings must be compared with 211 similar heere forms.
The total number of B spellings gratuitously introduced into the Paviers is 615. In view of the token number of twenty-one changes in the opposite direction, the presence of B's hand throughout the Paviers is evident. It is also important to note that the few aberrant non-B changes are not significantly grouped, and in no instance do they suggest the presence of another compositor.
The examination of the do, go, here spellings in the Pavier quartos shows the special significance of change, that is, the altering of copy in contrast to the following of copy. The spelling pattern of go/goe in A Midsummer Night's Dream is an excellent example of the relative importance of spelling change. The raw evidence in the Paviers is almost evenly divided between twenty go and seventeen goe. The direction of the changes from copy which produced this result tells a far different story. Of the four go spellings in copy, there is not one instance of change to goe. Of the thirty-three goe spellings in copy, however, almost half, sixteen, were changed to go. On the surface, the sixteen changes in thirty-three occurrences of goe might appear to express a certain degree of compositorial indifference to the spelling of this word. When viewed as a part of the overall pattern of B's spellings and in relation to the four go forms, all of which remain unchanged, this change of sixteen out of thirty-three goe to go is far from expressing indifference. Rather, it indicates as strongly as possible the compositor's preference for the go form. A single change is of much more significance in assessing a compositor's preferences than is a single case of his following copy. The sixteen changes of goe to go tell a great deal more about the compositor's preference than do the seventeen instances of following copy.

This examination of do, go, here spellings in the Paviers has accomplished two things. First, it has shown the care which must be employed in avoiding over-simplification in the expression of statistical spelling evidence. Spelling analysis must be done in conjunction with an examination, where possible, of copy spellings, and the possible influence of justification on variant spellings must be considered at all times. Secondly, the introduction of these refinements into the analysis of do, go, here spellings has brought convincing strength to the demonstration of the hypothesis that Jaggard's Compositor B did in fact set the whole of the text of the Pavier quartos.
II.
Armed with this warning against over-simplification and, at the same time, strengthened by the assurance that B's hand is in evidence throughout the Paviers, I collated all of the Pavier texts against their copy and compiled word counts for over 500 individual words. This mass of statistical evidence was then analyzed in terms of orthographic groups which display significant spelling features. Although I do not wish to reproduce these tedious reports of observed statistics, I propose to give here a list of the groups of words I examined and one sample report in order to make the nature of my study known to those who may be interested in consulting it.[13]
The groups of spellings considered were as follows:
- 1. Internal -A-/-AU-
- 2. Initial DE-/DI-
- 3. Internal Consonants: -D-/-DD-, -R-/-RR-, -T-/-TT-
- 4. Final -D/-DE
- 5. Initial E-/I- before -N-/-M-
- 6. Internal -EA-/-EE-
- 7. Internal -EA-/-EI-
- 8. Internal -EE-/-IE-
- 9. Final -E/-EE
- 10. Final -F/-FE
- 11. Final -G
- 12. Internal -I-/-Y-
- 13. Final -IE/-Y
- 14. Final -K/-CK/-QUE
- 15. Final -L
- 16. Final -M
- 17. Final -N
- 18. Internal -O-/-OO-
- 19. Final -P
- 20. Final -R
- 21. Final -S/-SSE
- 22. Final -T
- 23. Final -W
- 24. Final -X
- 25. Reflexive Pronouns
One of the largest of these groups of words is that involving -ie/-y endings. Before one can attempt to generalize about the eighty-eight

All of the following words display B's extremely strong preference for final -y in conjunction with a preceding vowel:
away | enjoy | pay |
betray | joy | pray |
bewray | lay | say |
boy | may | stay |
day | nay | way |
The following words ending in -ie/-y after penultimate letters b, c, d, g, k, l, m, n, p, r, t, and v regularly display B's preference for -y endings:
b: rugby | willingly | t: authority |
c: fancy | m: enemy | beauty |
mercy | n: any | charity |
d: already | company | city |
bloody | deny | cruelty |
body | honey | duty |
lady | many | fifty |
needy | money | forty |
ready | villany | guilty |
speedy | p: happy | honesty |
g: clergy | r: beggery | liberty |
k: lucky | carry | loyalty |
l: daily | contrary | majesty |
dearly | country | mighty |
early | cry | nativity |
fly | dowry | necessity |
heartily | every | petty |
heavenly | fury | pity |
holy | glory | pretty |
kingly | history | quality |
only | marry | safety |
presently | merry | thirty |
princely | misery | twenty |
quickly | sorry | university |
reply | tarry | v: envy |
suddenly | very | heavy |
truly | victory |

There are two consonants which do not fall into the above group and, in fact, display a distinctly contrasting pattern. These consonants are f and s. Defie stands in B's copy six times and all of these six occurrences are reproduced by B. B's copy spells satisfy with final -ie all five times it occurs. B follows copy for all of these forms. Signifie is so spelled in six (+J1) occurrences in copy. All of these forms are reproduced by B. All four (+J1) occurrences of busy are spelled busie both in copy and in B's texts. B reproduces the seven (+J1) curtesie spellings in copy, changes three (+J1) curtesie forms to courtesie, and reproduces three courtesie spellings. All seven easie spellings in copy are retained in B's texts. Heresie is reproduced from copy the three times the word occurs. One heresie form is set in place of copy's herisie. The seven occurrences of jealousy are spelled with final -ie both in copy and in B's texts. Lowsie is reproduced from copy three times and once changed to lowsy.
All of the words recorded for f and s plus -ie/-y display an uncanny pattern of strongly favoring final -ie in both copy and B's texts. It is likely that the anomalous -fie/-sie endings result from the use of the fi and long-s/i ligatures. Since all of the Pavier copy produced by various printers agrees with B's use of -fie/-sie in spite of the general -ie/-y treatment, the fi and si ligatures were apparently used with some consistency in order to make the supply of plain f's and s's in the case last as long as possible. The unanimity of the printers in following this procedure makes it appear to be a general convention for stretching the supply of type.
B displays, therefore, a special tolerance for final -ie after -f- and -s-. It is important to stress the word tolerance because this special feature of B's treatment of final -ie/-y cannot be termed a preference. Without being able to observe B's reaction to -fy and -sy in copy, we cannot equate his use of -fie/-sie with his demonstrable general preference for final -y as evidenced by the majority of the evidence given above. Indeed, the only change in that last group of -f- and -s- is that of one of the four lowsie forms to lowsy. Taken by itself, this shred of evidence points to the fact that B's preferential spelling was final -y but that his tolerance for ligatures with -ie was surprisingly strong. It must also be pointed out that B's tolerance for -fie and -sie is not simply a tendency to give in to copy spellings when they are weighted against his preferences

To summarize B's spelling patterns for words ending in -ie/-y, B consistently prefers final -y after preceding vowels and consonants with the exception of f and s. Although his preference for final -y involves both the retention of almost all -y endings and the introduction of -y endings for the vast majority of -ie forms in his copy, a limited percentage of -ie forms is carried over from copy into B's texts. These few -ie forms serve as an indication of copy spellings. For the only exceptions to this general rule, -fie and -sie, B displays an almost absolute tolerance for the -ie endings which are consistently found in copy.
An examination of compositor B's group spellings has revealed both the dangers of and benefits to be derived from the use of analogy in the analysis of compositorial spellings. Although compositor identification in the Folio has generally been limited to the use of variant spellings of individual words, other compositor studies have employed classes of words such as that formed by all words with -ie/-y endings. Since investigators have employed these word classes in compositor identification, it is of interest to examine the nature and extent of analogy in the overall spelling pattern of a compositor.
There are several large groups of words which B treats in a unified manner. His consistent use of final -e after penultimate -f- is of little significance because the vast majority of copy spellings are also -fe. B's consistent preference for -esse forms of words varying between -es and -esse is an individual habit which extends to all of the similar words recorded in the Paviers. From the evidence of the Pavier spellings, a reasonably safe projection can be made from the uniformity of B's preference for -esse. It is, however, not demonstrable that the very next word examined will not for some special reason depart from the pattern and display the opposite tendency. The discovery of B's clearly defined preference for deere might lead one to expect that B would also prefer the yeere spelling. In fact, however, B's preference for yeare is equally strong as his use of deere. Word classes cannot, therefore, be formed by the use of simple analogy.
Although word classes cannot be automatically formed of analogical words, Compositor B's spellings are far from being patternless. Consistent patterns can in fact be observed for groups of similar words.

There are many similar situations which show the importance of building up word classes of a more limited sort. Internal -ai-/-ay- spellings appear to be mixed until it is observed that B's spellings split into an -ayle group and a more general -ai- group. In this case, the subgroup displaying -ay- is formed by words having analogous endings. The other group is not governed by any common feature other than the internal -ai-. Internal -r-/-rr- variation, however, falls into two groups, both of which are composed of analogous words.
In summary, therefore, Compositor B's spelling patterns indicate that analogy often governed his treatment of various spellings. At the same time, however, untested analogy cannot be employed to define linguistic units for compositor study. The exact nature and extent of these analogous groups must be determined only as a result of the study of separate words.
Although analogy must be employed with extreme caution, one important factor, aberration, should not be permitted to interfere with the careful formation of word groups and classes. The Pavier statistics confirm a widely held hypothesis that for some unknown reason a given compositor will on rare occasions contradict even his strongest habit. The point here is that these aberrations should not be allowed to obscure the valid evidence of spelling patterns.
In the examination of do, go, and here, for example, B was seen to go against his strong do habit and set doe three times in the course of the Paviers in instances when he was affected neither by the need for justification nor by a copy doe spelling. These three aberrations in

To take another example, a word first analyzed in the Paviers, the following statistics represent the word count for only. The first column represents copy spellings and the second the spelling of those words in the Paviers.
- ONLIE — ONELY 1
- ONLY — ONELY 12
- ONELY — ONELY 33+J2
- ONELY — ONLY 1
Although the existence of these aberrations should not be allowed to obscure clearly observable general patterns, the fact that single aberrant spellings occur calls for caution in placing weight on any one individual spelling. To paraphrase Fredson Bowers commenting on aberration in the printing process, the phenomenon of an individual spelling is narrow enough "to introduce the possibility that a recognized . . . [spelling preference] is not being revealed but instead that we are examining only an aberration — some human variation or failure that must be regarded as producing a sport."[14]
III.
The examination of the Pavier spellings has also produced valuable statistics on many individual words of interest. Spellings which have been proposed by previous investigators as characteristic B spellings are here reviewed in the light of the information provided by the statistics covering B's work in the Paviers. All of the words that have been proposed by Satchell, Willoughby, Cauthen,[15] and Walker[16] as aids in identifying B's work have been observed in the Paviers. None of these investigators considered B's habit spellings in direct relation to copy, and all four of them were working before Hinman's isolation of Compositor E. When reference is make to these spelling analyses,

1. been
As a part of his attempt to demonstrate that all of Folio Lear was set by B, Cauthen presented the following table as evidence that B's preferential spelling was bin.
A Spellings | B Spellings | Lear Spellings |
beene (13) | beene (4) | beene (8) |
bene (3) | bene (6) | bene (1) |
bin (3) | bin (24) | bin (19) |


2. blood (bloody)
Compositor B displays a marked preference for -oo- forms of blood and bloody. Of the 103 (+J4) -ou- forms in copy, B changes sixty-three (+J4) to -oo-. At the same time, he retains sixty-one (+J5) of the sixty-six (+J5) -oo- spellings found in copy. It is interesting to note that in spite of his clear-cut preference for -oo-, Compositor B does allow four -ou- spellings in ten to come through into his texts. In all ten plays, there are only five abberant changes of -oo- to -ou- or about one in thirteen occurrences of the word.
3. brief
Cauthen's statistics suggest that B has a two to one preference for briefe over breefe. In the Paviers, only one justified -ee- form occurs in copy and this is followed by B. Of the twenty-one (+J3) -ie- forms in copy, B changes five to -ee- and retains sixteen (+J3) as -ie-. From this evidence, it would appear that any preference which these statistics point toward is for -ee- over -ie-. To complete the picture, it would be necessary to be able to observe B's treatment of several -ee- forms when found in copy.
4. choose
Alice Walker has suggested that B displays a preference for choose in contrast to A's chuse. In the limited evidence provided by the Paviers, B changes three of the five chuse forms in copy to choose. Of the three choose forms in copy, B retains two but sets one as chuse. Walker's suggestion that B favored choose is confirmed by the evidence in the Paviers.
5. cousin
Taking the -o-/-ou- variation as the operative feature, the evidence provided by the Paviers fails to confirm Willoughby's and Walker's suggestion that B preferred -o- forms over -ou- spellings. Of the nine (+J1) -ou- spellings in copy, B retains eight and changes only one (+J1). Of the eleven (+J3) -o- spellings in copy, B retains ten (+J3) and changes only one.

6. dear
The variation of -ea-/-ee- is the operative feature in dear. Of the sixty (+J12) -ea- forms found in copy, B changes twenty-three (+J6) to -ee-. He retains all thirty-seven (+J7) -ee- forms in copy. These statistics point clearly to his preference for deere.
7. devil
Compositor B expresses a strong preference for the di- form of devil. Of the eighteen (+J7) de- forms in copy, B changes seventeen (+J6) to di-. Only one (+J1) non-preferential de- spelling slips through from copy into B's text. All twenty-seven (+J9) di- forms in copy are retained by B.
8. forfeit
Forfeit provides another example, like been, of a situation where multiple alternative spellings [forfait(e), forfet, and forfeit(e)] were available to B. In this instance, he displays a double preference for forfet and forfeit(e) and completely rejects the third alternative. At the same time, he fails to display any single clear-cut preference for either of the two forms which he does use.
9. grant
Walker's suggestion that B preferred the grant form is strongly confirmed by the Pavier spellings. He changes every one of the eight (+J1) copy graunt spellings to grant and retains all twelve (+J2) copy grant forms.
10. grief
Walker included greefe in her list of B's preferred spellings. Of the thirty-eight (+J5) griefe spellings in copy, B changes seventeen (+J1) to greefe. Both greefe forms in copy are retained. The greefe preference is confirmed.
11. heart
B displays a marked preference for the -ea- form of heart. Of the ninety-two (+J13) hart forms in copy, B changes seventy-three (+J6) to -ea-. He retains 111 (+J21) of the 112 (+J24) -ea- forms found in copy. There is only one (+J3) anomalous change of -ea- to -a-. B's -ea- preference is clearly indicated.
12. heaven
Alice Walker observed that B preferred uncapitalized heaven up until Macbeth in the Folio. In the Paviers, B changes eight of the nine capitalized Heaven forms to lower case and reproduces thirty-six (+J2) uncapitalized forms found in copy.

13. honey
Alice Walker first observed B's preference for the hony spelling of this word. He changes all three honey spellings in copy to hony and reproduces the four short forms which stand in his copy. There is one anomalous change of hony to honny.
14. hour
Cauthen and Walker both indicate that B's preferred spelling was houre. Of the thirteen (+J2) -ow- spellings of hour in copy, B changes eleven (+J2) to -ou-. One hower and one houre-glasse spellings are reproduced by B from his copy. He retains all thirty-four (+J4) -ou- forms found in copy. The Pavier evidence indicates that B's preference for -ou- was quite strong.
15. "lushious"
The one occurrence of lushious in both copy and Pavier texts is included in this discussion because Alice Walker put great weight upon the coincidence of the lushious spelling in Q1 and Folio Othello. She used this coincidence as evidence that Folio Othello was derived from a marked copy of Q1 (1622). "It is ridiculous to suppose that anomalies like 'timerous' (I.i.76), 'lushious' (I.iii.346) and 'pudled' (III.iv.144) passed from a fair copy, via a prompt book, to a manuscript prepared by a book-keeper with (at best) only half an eye on the prompt-book, and so into Okes's quarto, thereby representing a common legacy in the quarto and Folio from some common ancestor."[18] If Miss Walker had checked the Concordance, she would have found two recorded uses of luscious, in the Othello passage in question (I.iii.354 — — Globe numbering) and in A Midsummer Night's Dream (II.i.251). Both Midsummer Q1 (1600) and the Pavier Q2 (1619) contain the lushious spelling (C2v in both Qq). Far from being anomalous, the lushious form is common to the compositors of Q1 Midsummer, Q1 Othello, and Jaggard's B. It is interesting to note that the Midsummer lushious is changed to luscious on N3v in the Folio. Hinman assigns this page to Compositor C.[19] The point of this example is that any common spelling, however "anomalous" it may appear, must be used with extreme caution in attempting to demonstrate direct derivation of one text from another.
16. madam
Alice Walker suggested that madam is a characteristic B spelling whereas madame is characteristic of Compositor A. Disregarding the

17. mistress
Five variant spellings of mistress are found in the copy used for the Paviers: mistres (eight +J1), mistris (twenty-two +J10), mistresse (forty-seven +J11), mistrisse (one +J1), and misteris (thirty-five +J5). Of these five spellings, B prefers mistris and mistresse and rejects the other three. Although Alice Walker assigned mistresse to A and mistris to B as characteristic spellings, B's treatment of the word in the Paviers indicates that he employed both forms. On the surface, simple numerical superiority does favor the -is form, but this is a result of the numerical superiority of -is forms in copy.
18. power
Cauthen observed that B's texts contained both power (18) and powre (7).[20] In the Paviers, however, B changes the two powre forms in copy to power and retains all twenty-six of the power spellings which stand in his copy, thus indicating a preference for power.
19. prithee
Alice Walker noted B's constant use of the spelling prethee in the early sections of the Folio. In the Paviers, B normalized a total of twenty-six (+J6) occurrences of ten different spellings of prithee all to his preferential prethee. He follows one copy prithee, sets one justified copy prithe as prithee, and follows one copy prethe. B's habit in the Paviers coincides, therefore, with Walker's observation of his treatment of prithee in the early parts of the Folio.
20. proud
The Paviers confirm Walker's assignment of proud as B's preferential form. B changes the one copy prowd and one (+J1) proude to proud and retains twelve occurrences of the latter form from copy.

21. scarce
The evidence supplied by the Paviers confirms the observation that B had a strong preference for the -se form of scarce. He changes all eighteen (+J2) -ce forms in copy to -se and retains all five -se forms which stand in copy.
22. traitor
Willoughby and Walker both stress B's preference for traitor over traytor, and the Paviers confirm this preference. Of the twenty-five (+J4) traytor forms in copy, B changes twenty-five (+J1) to traitor. All thirty-five (+J3) -i- forms in copy are retained by B. The only one (+J1) traytor form in B's texts is derived from copy.
23. vile
Cauthen assigned the vile spelling to Compositor A and vilde to B. B changes all five vild spellings in copy to vilde and one copy vilde to vile. He reproduces the two (+J1) other vilde spellings found in copy.
24. wee'l (etc.)
Alice Walker suggested that B had a distinctive preference for wee'l (etc.) spellings as opposed to wee'le (etc.). The following discussion breaks these forms down by person.
The various compositors who set the texts used for Pavier copy and Compositor B display a surprising unanimity on the spelling of I'll. There are 124 (+J13) instances of ile in both copy and Pavier texts. One aberrant yle form found in copy is changed to ile. B's one deviation from the standard form occurs in a justified line in which he sets I'le for Ile.
He'll, she'll, we'll, and you'll occur in sufficient numbers to demonstrate B's strong preferences for final 'l forms of these words. Several variant factors are involved in these words. The use of the apostrophe, the ending (-l or -le) and the doubling (in the case of he'll, she'll and we'll) of the first e are all separate alternatives operative for these words. B's uniform preferences are for the doubling of the initial e, the use of the apostrophe, and the omission of the final -e.
Of the eighteen (+J2) times B encounters heele in copy, he changes eighteen (+J1) of these forms to his preferential hee'l. In the one justified instance, B changes heele to heel. In the two instances in which he encountered hee'le both are changed to hee'l. The one contraction introduced by B changes he will to hee'l. One aberrant hele in B's copy comes through as an unusual heele in his text.
Of the five sheele forms in copy, B changes four to shee'l and sets one as shee'll. He changes all four copy shee'le spellings to his preferential

We'll is a more complex word. B displays a preference for wee'l but is subject to the influence of copy and more prone to deviate from his strict preference. B changes twenty-nine (+J3) weele forms to wee'l. At the same time, he changes two justified weele forms to wee'll, fourteen (+J2) weele forms to weel, three weele forms to weell, one justified weele form to weel and retains eight (+J3) weele forms found in copy. If the three wele spellings in copy, B changes one to his apparent preference wee'l but the other two to weele. The one we'le form is changed to his preferred wee'l as are the eight (+J1) occurrences of wee'le. The two wee'l forms are reproduced from copy. Of the two (+J1) weel spellings in copy, one (+J1) is changed to B's preferential wee'l and one is retained. Both the one weell and the one we wil which are contracted by B become weel.
Compositor B displays a strong preference for you'l by changing twenty (+J3) youle spellings to this form. Only twice does he reproduce youle from copy and in one unusual instance sets youle in place of his preferred you'l. One other you'l in copy is retained, and the two you'le forms are shortened to you'l.
With the exception of the Ile which is uniformly spelled thusly, Compositor B displays a consistent spelling preference in favor of hee'l, wee'l, and you'l. The only variation comes in his tendency to depart occasionally from his strict habit with respect to wee'l.
One additional word can be considered with this group. B's treatment of contracted he is is as follows. Twice he sets hee's for copy he is. Of the eleven (+J4) hees in copy, B sets six (+J4) as hee's and five as he's. The one he's in copy is changed to hee's. Of the seventeen (+J3) hee's spellings in copy, B follows copy seventeen (+J2) times and changes one hee's to hee is in a justified line where these words stand at the very end of the line. B's preference is clearly for the hee's spelling.
25. widow
The Pavier evidence confirms Alice Walker's suggestion that B's preference is for widdow. Of the seven (+J1) -d- forms in copy, B changes three to -dd-. He retains eight (+J2) of the ten (+J2) -dd- forms found in copy. In addition, B drops the final -e found on one form in copy.

26. year
B's preference is clearly for the yeare spelling. He changes all seven (+J3) -ee- spellings in copy to -ea- and preserves twenty (+J4) of the twenty-one (+J4) -ea- forms found in copy.
27. young
Alice Walker assigns the yong spellings to B and young to A. The evidence in the Paviers shows that B's treatment of young does not display a marked preference for either form. Of the thirty-six (+J18) -ou- spellings in copy, B changes eight (+J6) to -o-. On the other hand, he changes eleven (+J6) of the twenty-eight (+J8) -o- spellings to -ou-. The number of changes of -o- to -ou- has a slight numerical superiority over -ou- to -o-, but no clear-cut habit can be observed.
It is worth noting the extent to which previous investigators dealing with Compositor B in the Folio and working without the ability to distinguish E from B as well as without reference to copy have determined B's preferential spellings as they are revealed in the Paviers. Four of the five test words proposed by Willoughby as means of identifying B are in fact strong B preferences.[21] Twenty-one of the forms proposed by Alice Walker as characteristic B spellings have been studied in the Paviers, and seventeen of the twenty-one are strong preferential spellings there. Two conclusions can be drawn from these results. First, the limited amount of text set by E and the closeness of his overall spelling pattern to that of B have not obscured B's spellings from previous investigators. Second, the fact that seventeen of B's Folio habits are confirmed in the Paviers indicates the extent to which set habits remained fixed. Although it will be necessary for later investigations to analyze B's Folio spellings fully, the preliminary indications are that the Paviers give an excellent idea of B's spellings. This is not to say, however, that any one individual B spelling as observed in the Paviers can be assumed to be his fixed preferential spelling. The general texture of B's spellings in the Paviers, however, should be reflected in his work in the Folio.
It is also important to note that the various words here discussed display all of the various logical possibilities for a compositor's treatment of variant spellings. B is seen to display strong preferences for one, two, and even three variant spellings of given words and can display indifference which appears neither to be influenced by a weak preference nor to depend on copy forms.

IV.
My examination of the Pavier spellings was undertaken as a result of the hypothesis that an analysis of compositorial spellings can provide not merely the basis for accurately identifying a compositor's work by his preferential spellings, but also the means of seeing through the workman to his copy. In many situations, it would be extremely advantageous, once the compositor has been identified, to penetrate the layer of his spellings and go behind the man to the copy from which he was setting his type. Compositorial identification has concentrated on the discovery of forms like do, go, and heere which are signs indicating that the text in question is B's work. The hypothesis in question stated, however, that if a new group of spellings can be discovered which contains not only these compositorial habits and preferences, but also spellings which reproduce copy spellings as well, there will be a means of seeing through that compositor to his copy. The examination of the Pavier spellings has produced the evidence to verify this hypothesis and has further shown that even the identifying badges, strong preferences, can be used to get at copy spellings.
There are various types of words which can produce information about copy. The ideal word is one that has two or more variant spellings which occur in mixed groups and are mechanically reproduced by the compositor in question. Such words which then directly depict the copy spelling at every occurrence turn out to be extremely rare. The only examples which are invariably reliable in the Paviers are art(20)/arte(2), brier(2)/bryer(2), marvell(4)/marveile(5+J1), and pallace(4)/pallas(1). All four of these words occur in the Paviers with limited frequency, but they serve to demonstrate that words can be found which will vary in direct relation to copy. Oh is the best example of a high frequency word which varies in extremely close relation to copy spelling. Of the 361 mixed O (244+J45) and Oh (177+J20) forms in B's copy, 341 reflect copy spellings. When one encounters an O or Oh spelling in B's Pavier texts, therefore, the odds are better than fifteen to one that B's spelling is the spelling which stood in copy. Words of this type which can be said to express compositorial indifference are unfortunately relatively scarce and are, therefore, only one contributing factor to the search for spellings which reflect copy. When they do occur, however, the evidential value is extremely strong.
At the other end of the scale are strong habits and preferences which, if viewed from a new perspective and not as identifying badges indicating a given compositor's presence, can contribute clues to copy spellings. Take, for example, do, the strongest of B's preferential spellings. In all of the Paviers, B violates his strong preference fifteen times

Between the words expressing compositorial indifference and those which display a strong preference are all shades of a spectrum. Among these words are many which can be termed weak preference words. These words are those for which B displays some favorite spelling or spellings but neither a very strong habit nor indifference. Dear is an ideal example of a weak preference word. B clearly displays a preference for the deere spelling by following every one of the thirty-seven (+J7) deere spellings in copy and changing twenty-three (+J6) of the sixty (+J12) deare spellings in copy to deere. It is the thirty-seven (+J6) non-preferential deare spellings which B reproduces in his texts from his copy, however, which are of importance for copy study. B's preference was weak, and he allowed it to be subordinated to copy spellings in these numerous instances so that all of these deare spellings in his texts directly reflect copy spellings. We are, so to speak, able to see through B back to copy when we see a deare in his text. Dear is spoken of as an ideal example of a weak preference, copy-reflecting word because the complete absence of aberrant -ee- to -ea- changes makes the deare absolutely reliable as an indication of the copy spelling. Every single one of the thirty-seven (+J6) -ea- forms in B's texts directly reflects an -ea- form in copy.
There are, of course, many gradations of preferences ranging from words that approach the strong habits like do, go, and heere to those which fade into examples of compositorial indifference. In addition, many of these weak preference words are not like deare in being 100% reliable. Perfect reliability is not, however, a requisite for words to use in copy study. Take, for example, the weak preference word credit. B's demonstrable preference is for the credite spelling, but he sets credit seven (+J1) times. Of these seven times, six reflect six of the thirteen credit spellings in copy and one represents an aberrant change of the supposedly preferred credite to credit. This one aberrancy does not destroy the value of the word for the purpose of getting at copy since the odds are still six to one that credit in the Pavier text will reflect credit in copy.
The following compilation includes over 100 words which, within the Pavier texts, demonstrably reflect the characteristics of copy. Here,

Words have been included in this list down to a 67% degree of reliability. This 67% figure in itself may not sound as strong as when it is expressed in the form of two-to-one odds that the copy-reflecting spelling does indicate the spelling in copy. This percentage is calculated only for the unjustified occurrences of the spellings in the third column so as to remove any possible contamination from the exigencies of justification. Since it is impossible to draw lines dividing the spectrum of copy-reflecting words into different groups, the percentage of reliability must be considered in conjunction with the number of times the preferential (column two) and copy-reflecting (column three) spellings occur in B's texts in order to evaluate the significance of a specific spelling as a form which reflects copy. Because of the dangers inherent in dealing with statistically small samples, even this rough indication of relative percentages of reliability has been omitted for words which do not occur at least ten times in the Paviers.

Word | B's Preference(s) | Spellings Reflecting Copy | |
aboard | aboard (5) | aboord (4+J1) | 100% |
act | acte (12+J3) | act (4) | 100% |
adieu | final -u (10) | final -w (2+J2) | 100% |
aid | medial -i- (12+J1) | medial -y- (3) | 100% |
alack | alacke (18) | alack (4) | 75% |
already | already (12) | alreadie (1) | 100% |
always | alwayes (7+J1) | alwaies (5+J1) | 100% |
and | and (General Rule) | & (4+J4) | 100% |
angel | angel (5+J1) | angell (5) | 100% |
answer | answer (43+J7) | final -re (12+J3) | 83% |
approach | approch (6+J1) | approach (5) | 100% |
art | _____ | art (20) | 100% |
arte (2) | 100% | ||
bar | barre (9) | bar (1) | 100% |
battle | battell (12+J4) | battle (1) | 100% |
battaile (7+J1) | 86% | ||
been | _____ | bin (18+J10) | 72% |
behind | behinde (17+J2) | behind (4+J3) | 75% |
bishop | byshop (19) | bishop (9) | 100% |
blind | blinde (14+J5) | blind (2+J2) | 100% |
blood | blood (97+J8) | bloud (32) | 84% |
bloody | bloody (27+J1) | medial -ou- (13) | 100% |
bow | bow (4) | bowe (1) | |
box | box (1) | boxe (2+J1) | |
briar | _____ | bryer (2) | |
brier (2) | |||
brief | breefe (5) | briefe (12+J3) | 100% |
charity | charity (12+J3) | charitie (1) | 100% |
cheer | cheere (10+J2) | cheare (3) | 100% |
chief | cheefe (8+J1) | chiefe (1+J2) | 100% |
choice | choise (21+J1) | internal -y- (4) | 100% |
choose | choose (5) | chuse (3) | |
clear | cleere (9) | cleare (11+J1) | 82% |
coin | coine (4) | coyne (2) | |
command | command (17+J2) | commaund (1) | 100% |
courtesy | courtesie (6+J1) | curtesie (7+J1) | 100% |
cousin | _____ | internal -ou- (9) | 89% |
internal -o- (11+J4) | 90% | ||
credit | credite (8+J3) | credit (7+J1) | 86% |
crown | crowne (21+J3) | crown (1+J1) | 100% |
cruel | cruell (13+J1) | cruel (1) | 100% |
dear | deere (60+J13) | deare (37+J6) | 100% |
deed | deede (8+J2) | deed (14+J1) | 71% |
deny | deny (13+J3) | denie (1) | 100% |
devil | divell (44+J15) | devill (1+J1) | 100% |
do | do (573+J103) | doe (15+J21) | 80% |
duchess | final -esse (9+J3) | final -es (1+J1) | 100% |
duty | duty (14+J1) | dutie (1+J1) | 100% |
enemy | enemy (12+J1) | enemie (3) | 100% |
error | error (7+J1) | errour (1) | |
extreme | extreme (12) | extreame (2) | 100% |
fancy | fancy (4) | fancie (1) | |
forth | foorth (25+J3) | forth (16+J2) | 87% |
fury | fury (9) | furie (3) | 100% |

Word | B's Preference(s) | Spellings Reflecting Copy | |
girl | gyrle (6) | girle (1+J2) | |
go | go (231+J40) | goe (73+J32) | 93% |
grief | greefe (19+J1) | griefe (21+J4) | 100% |
grieve | greeve (19) | grieve (4+J3) | 75% |
guard | guard (13+J1) | gard (3) | 100% |
guess | guesse (5) | gesse (2) | |
hear | heare (19+J2) | hear (1) | 100% |
heart | heart (184+J27) | hart (20+J10) | 95% |
herald | herald (7) | herauld (5) | 80% |
here | heere (345+J44) | here (150+J41) | 91% |
heretic | hereticke (2) | heretike (6) | |
high | high (7+J2) | hi (y) e (9) | 100% |
hour | houre (45+J6) | internal -ow- (2) | 100% |
intreat | initial en- (17+J2) | initial in- (13+J1) | 92% |
final -te (22+J2) | final -t (4+J1) | 100% | |
jest | jeast (4) | jest (3) | |
keys | keyes (3) | keies (1) | |
kiss | kisse (15) | kis (2) | 100% |
liberty | liberty (4) | libertie (1) | |
loud | _____ | lowd (4) | 75% |
loud (7) | 86% | ||
mad | mad (13+J2) | madde (1+J1) | 100% |
madam | medial -d- (78+J12) | medial -dd- (1+J1) | 100% |
maid | internal -i- (25+J7) | internal -y- (2) | 100% |
majesty | majesty (47+J3) | majestie (2+J1) | 100% |
marvel | _____ | marvell (4) | 100% |
marvaile (5+J1) | 100% | ||
master | master (121+J19) | maister (4+J3) | 100% |
mighty | mighty (15+J3) | mightie (1) | 100% |
mind | minde (61+J10) | mind (4+J3) | 100% |
mock | mocke (9) | mock (1) | 100% |
month | month (6+J1) | moneth (2) | |
moral | morall (4) | morrall (1) | |
murder | _____ | internal -d- (39+J3) | 80% |
internal -th- (34+J3) | 79% | ||
near | neere (45+J4) | neare (2) | 100% |
need | neede (9+J2) | need (13+J4) | 69% |
noise | noise (5+J4) | noyse (3) | 100% |
oh | _____ | o (244+J45) | 99% |
oh (117+J20) | 85% | ||
old | olde (21+J18) | final -d (42+J16) | 93% |
palace | palace (3+J2) | pallace (4) | 100% |
pallas (1) | 100% | ||
pity | internal -tt- (42+J7) | internal -t- (2+J1) | 100% |
poison | poyson (8) | poison (3) | 67% |
quick | quicke (6+J4) | quick (2) | 100% |
relieve | internal -ee- (11) | internal -ie- (1) | 100% |
run | run (16+J2) | runne (13+J7) | 100% |
sail | internal -y- (10+J2) | internal -i- (4+J1) | 100% |
seven | seven (9) | seaven (2) | 100% |
show(s) | medial -e- (75+J8) | medial -o- (13) | 100% |
singular: | final -w (60+J3) | final -we (2+J1) | 100% |
son | sonne (87+J14) | son (5+J3) | 80% |
sorry | internal -rr- (5) | internal -r- (3+J1) |

Word | B's Preference(s) | Spellings Reflecting Copy | |
sudden | _____ | internal -u- (4+J2) | 100% |
internal -o- (11+J3) | 90% | ||
summer | summer (6+J2) | sommer (7+J1) | 100% |
sun | sunne (23+J1) | sun (5) | 80% |
together | together (12+J2) | togither (3) | 100% |
traitor | internal -i- (59+J4) | internal -y- (1+J1) | 100% |
truly | truely (7+J2) | truly (2+J1) | 100% |
try | try (6+J2) | trie (2) | 100% |
unless | unlesse (18) | unles (1) | 100% |
victory | victory (6) | victorie (2) | |
war | single r (5) | double rr (22) | 100% |
wax | wax (4+J2) | waxe (2+J5) | 100% |
we'll | wee'l (42+J5) | final -le (10+J3) | 100% |
wind | winde (34+J10) | wind (3) | 100% |
witness | witnesse (15+J4) | witnes (1) | 100% |
you'll | you'l (23+J3) | youle (3) | 67% |
young | _____ | internal -o- (25+J8) | 68% |
internal -ou- (39+J18) | 72% |
The forms in this table which are reflections of copy constitute the class of words hypothesized to exist at the beginning of this study. The large number of spellings which belong to this class and the consistently high degree of their reliability at reflecting copy spellings show that words which reflect copy are an important part of the overall pattern of B's spellings. Compositor B has more than simple preferential spellings; he has a complex pattern of strong preferences, weak preferences, tolerances, and even mechanical indifference. Time after time under the pressure of copy, he sets non-preferential spellings about which he was relatively indifferent and thereby allows his texts to transmit the evidence of spellings which lie behind them. No longer must the person investigating a B text look only for do and go and heere spellings. He will look for doe, goe, here, youle, wind and so on, which by long odds are indications of copy spellings. Using a large number of the forms which reflect copy spellings, we can now start to assemble from the evidence in B's texts a projection of the texture of copy spellings.
Until the Pavier statistics are carefully extended by word counts on control situations in the Folio, no thoroughgoing application of this new method for determining spelling features of unknown or disputed copy can be attempted. As a brief test of the validity of this theoretical technique, however, let us turn to the first text occurring in the 1623 Folio which was set by B from known copy and perform a formal experiment in hopes of indicating the validity of the technique. Such an experiment can offer a formal demonstration under laboratory-like conditions of this new method for determining the characteristics of

According to Hinman's study, Compositor B set the last six pages of Love's Labors Lost in the Folio (M4r-M6v).[22] Examining these six Folio pages, we find the following forms which occur in unjustified lines and should, on the basis of the results of the Pavier analysis, indicate copy spellings.
- alwaies 1
- bin 1
- deare 3
- griefe 1
- hart 3
- jest 5
- O 12
- show 3
- sodaine 1
- yong 1
The experimental demonstration now depends on the confirmation or denial of the existence of these predicted phenomena by checking the control text, Love's Labors Lost Q1 (1598).[23] Checking these predicted spellings against the actual copy, we find that twenty-nine of the thirty-one or 93% of the projected spellings are in fact the spellings which stood in copy. Here is strong evidence that the hitherto unrecognized group of copy-reflecting spellings within B's total spelling system functions as an indication of copy. The strength of this experiment cannot be overemphasized. Starting with an hypothesis theoretically proposing the existence of a class of spellings which could be used as indications of copy spellings, a class of copy-reflecting spellings was constituted from the evidence derived from the control situation in the Paviers. The Folio sample was then examined for the presence of these newly discovered forms, and once they were found, yet another control confirmed the validity of the spellings themselves as reflections of copy and the validity of the technique of projecting copy spellings

It has now been demonstrated that a portion of a compositor's total spelling pattern is composed of spellings which directly reflect the spellings found in the compositor's copy. Although many investigators have conjectures about the origins and characteristics of copy used for various printed texts, the discovery and use of what are here called copy-reflecting spellings is the first procedure for determining features of copy. Many formerly vexing problems should be resolved by a thorough implementation of this method. After further investigation of spellings which reflect copy in Folio texts set by B from known copy, the road will be open to solve disputed problems like the possibility of the use of an annotated hybrid of Q1 (1608) and Q2 (1619) as copy for Folio Lear. Although authorities agree that the Folio Lear text was set from an annotated quarto, there is no agreement on the exact nature of copy. Using B's copy-reflecting spellings, one may be able to reliably project various features of the copy from B's share of the Folio text and then by inspection see which of the quartos matches the projection. In the case of texts set from MSS, these spellings which depend on copy spellings may provide the first substantial evidence concerning the specific physical features of copy. This new method, therefore, makes a start at enabling textual investigators to tackle one of the most vexing problems in establishing texts of English Renaissance drama, the nature of the lost MS copy behind extant printed texts. More specifically, when the technique has been applied to all of the workmen in the Shakespeare Folio, we may then possess an excellent tool for getting closer to the copy behind the texts of important Shakespearean plays that were printed for the first time in the Folio. The day may come when we can apply these techniques to texts set from Shakespearean foul papers and arrive at a far more intimate knowledge of the orthographical features of Shakespearean holograph than is at all possible today.
Notes
Charlton Hinman, "The Prentice Hand in the Tragedies of the Shakespeare First Folio: Compositor E," Studies in Bibliography, IX (1957), 3-20.
Charlton Hinman, The Printing and Proof-reading of the First Folio of Shakespeare (1963), I, 180-226.
It is interesting to note that the stability of Jaggard's compositorial staff is attested to by the evidence presented by D. F. McKenzie, "A List of Printers' Apprentices, 1605-1640," Studies in Bibliography, XIII (1960), 125. At the time of the setting of the Paviers, the newest workman recorded in Jaggard's shop was Laurence Yardsley, bound in 1614. See this table also for the identification of Folio Compositor E as John Leason, bound to Jaggard on November 4, 1622.
For a convenient review of the unmasking of the Pavier quartos, see E. K. Chambers, William Shakespeare: A Study of Facts and Problems (1930), I, 133-137 and his bibliographical references, 127.
For these identifications, see W. W. Greg, A Bibliography of English Printed Drama to the Restoration (1939-1959), 4 vols., passim.
D. F. McKenzie, "Compositor B's Role in The Merchant of Venice Q2 (1619)," Studies in Bibliography, XII (1959), 75-90.
For a discussion of the crucial distinction between qualitative and quantitative evidence, see Fredson Bowers, Bibliography and Textual Criticism (1964), pp. 194-195.
For the demonstration of the effect of justification on the Pavier spellings, see my note, "The Influence of Justification on Spelling in Jaggard's Compositor B," Studies in Bibliography, XX (1967), 235-239.
My complete study, with the discussions of these orthographic groups and a statistical appendix of word counts, is available as a University of Virginia dissertation (1966).
I. B. Cauthen, Jr., "Compositor Determination in the First Folio King Lear," Studies in Bibliography, V (1952-3), 73-80, especially 78.
Alice Walker, Textual Problems in the First Folio (Cambridge, 1953), p. 9 and "Compositor Determination and Other Problems in Shakespearean Texts," Studies in Bibliography, VII (1955), 14, note 8. The spelling tests employed by McKenzie and others are all derived from the previous investigations listed here.
See Charlton Hinman, "Spellings, Cases, and Compositors," The Printing and Proof-reading of the First Folio of Shakepeare (1963), I, 180-226, passim.
It must be remembered that Cauthen's investigations were contaminated by the then unknown Compositor E.
W. W. Greg, The Shakespeare First Folio: Its Bibliographical and Textual History (1955), p. 223.

![]() | | ![]() |